ducemid歌曲

mid歌曲  时间:2021-02-23  阅读:()
DopaminemodulatestherewardexperienceselicitedbymusicLauraFerreria,b,c,1,2,ErnestMas-Herrerod,e,1,RobertJ.
Zatorred,e,PabloRipollésf,AlbaGomez-Andresa,b,HelenaAlicarta,GuillemOlivéa,b,JosepMarco-Pallarésa,b,g,RosaM.
Antonijoanh,i,MartaVallei,j,3,JordiRibak,3,andAntoniRodriguez-Fornellsa,b,l,2,3aCognitionandBrainPlasticityUnit,BellvitgeBiomedicalResearchInstitute,L'HospitaletdeLlobregat,08907Barcelona,Spain;bDepartmentofCognition,DevelopmentandEducationPsychology,UniversityofBarcelona,08035Barcelona,Spain;cLaboratoired'EtudedesMécanismesCognitifs,UniversitéLumièreLyon2,69676Lyon,France;dMontrealNeurologicalInstitute,McGillUniversity,Montreal,QCH3A2B4,Canada;eInternationalLaboratoryforBrain,MusicandSoundResearch,Montreal,QCH2V4P3,Canada;fDepartmentofPsychology,NewYorkUniversity,NewYork,NY10003;gInstituteofNeuroscience,UniversityofBarcelona,08035Barcelona,Spain;hCentred'InvestigaciódeMedicaments,ServeideFarmacologiaClínica,HospitaldelaSantaCreuiSantPau,08041Barcelona,Spain;iDepartamentdeFarmacologiaiTerapèutica,UniversitatAutònomadeBarcelona,08193Barcelona,Spain;jPharmacokinetic/PharmacodynamicModelingandSimulation,SantPauInstitutofBiomedicalResearch,08041Barcelona,Spain;kDepartmentofNeuropsychologyandPsychopharmacology,MaastrichtUniversity,6229Maastricht,TheNetherlands;andlInstitucióCatalanadeRecercaiEstudisAvanats,08010Barcelona,SpainEditedbySolomonH.
Snyder,TheJohnsHopkinsUniversitySchoolofMedicine,Baltimore,MD,andapprovedDecember14,2018(receivedforreviewJuly12,2018)Understandinghowthebraintranslatesastructuredsequenceofsounds,suchasmusic,intoapleasantandrewardingexperienceisafascinatingquestionwhichmaybecrucialtobetterunderstandtheprocessingofabstractrewardsinhumans.
Previousneuroimagingfindingspointtoachallengingroleofthedopaminergicsysteminmusic-evokedpleasure.
However,thereisalackofdirectevidenceshowingthatdopaminefunctioniscausallyrelatedtothepleasureweexperiencefrommusic.
Weaddressedthisproblemthroughadoubleblindwithin-subjectpharmacologicaldesigninwhichwedirectlymanipulateddopaminergicsynapticavailabilitywhilehealthyparticipants(n=27)wereengagedinmusiclistening.
Weorallyadministratedtoeachparticipantadopamineprecursor(levo-dopa),adopamineantagonist(risperidone),andaplacebo(lactose)inthreedifferentsessions.
Wedemonstratethatlevodopaandris-peridoneledtooppositeeffectsinmeasuresofmusicalpleasureandmotivation:whilethedopamineprecursorlevodopa,comparedwithplacebo,increasedthehedonicexperienceandmusic-relatedmotivationalresponses,risperidoneledtoareductionofboth.
Thisstudyshowsacausalroleofdopamineinmusicalpleasureandindicatesthatdopaminergictransmissionmightplaydifferentoradditiverolesthantheonespostulatedinaffectiveprocessingsofar,particularlyinabstractcognitiveactivities.
music|dopamine|reward|pleasure|motivationAfascinatingaspectofhumansistheircapacitytoexperiencefeelingsofpleasurefromhighlycomplexpatternsofaudi-toryorvisualstimulationsuchasmusicandartwork(1–4).
In-triguingly,asitisthecaseformusic,theseactivitiesdonotprovidesurvivalvalues,asprimarypleasures(suchasfoodorsex)do,thusraisingquestionsabouttheultimategoalofthereward-relatedsignalstheycaninduceinmosthumansandtheneuralcircuitsunderlyingsuchparticularpleasure.
Previousresearchhasconsistentlyshownthatmusic-evokedpleasureisaccompaniedbyphysiologicalchangesintheauton-omousnervoussystem,aswellasmodulationofthemesolimbicrewardpathway,whicharesimilartothosefoundinresponsetoprimary(suchassexorfood)andsecondaryrewards(e.
g.
,money)(refs.
5–17;seealso,refs.
18and19).
Notably,aPETstudy(11)foundthat,similartotheprocessingofbiologicallyrelevantrewards,preferredmusicinducesdopaminereleaseinstriatalregions,particularlyinthenucleusaccumbens(NAcc)andthecaudate.
Thesefindingshaveledtoamodelwherebythere-cruitmentofdopaminergiccircuitsbymusic—throughcommuni-cationwithsensoryandcognitiveareasinvolvedintheprocessingofmusicalinformation—wouldresultintochangesinemotionalintensityandarousal,leadingtopleasurableandrewardingfeelings(20–23).
Thisviewchallengespreviousevidencefromprimaryrewardsconductedinrodents,wheredopaminergicmanipulationsshowaclearroleofdopamineinmotivationandlearning,butacontroversialfunctioninregulatinghedonicresponsesinprimaryrewardssuchasfood.
Indeed,thepleasurablecomponentofrewardhasbeenassociatedwithhedonichotspotsintheNAccregulatedbyopioids,ratherthandopaminergictransmission(23).
However,exceptforthestudyofSalimpooretal.
(11),mostre-searchonmusicalpleasurehasreliedonindirectmeasuresofneu-ronalactivation,withnospecificityforneurotransmittersystemsthatmaybeinvolved,andthustheirinterpretationabouttheactualneurochemistrysupportingmusicalpleasurehastobetakenwithcaution.
Inaddition,thereisnodirectevidenceshowingthatdo-paminefunctioniscausallyrelatedtomusic-evokedpleasure.
In-deed,mostofthestudiesconductedrelyoncorrelationalmethods,suchasneuroimaging,oronchemicallynonspecificbrainstimula-tionmethods(24).
Indeed,sofarnostudieshaveshownthatdirectmanipulationofsynapticdopaminergicavailabilitycanmodulatemusicallyinducedpleasure.
Thus,itremainselusivewhetherdopa-minereleaseandtheengagementofdopaminergiccircuitsSignificanceIneverydaylifehumansregularlyseekparticipationinhighlycomplexandpleasurableexperiencessuchasmusiclistening,singing,orplaying,thatdonotseemtohaveanyspecificsurvivaladvantage.
Thequestionaddressedhereistowhatextentdopaminergictransmissionplaysadirectroleintherewardexperience(bothmotivationalandhedonic)inducedbymusic.
Wereportthatpharmacologicalmanipulationofdopa-minemodulatesmusicalresponsesinbothpositiveandnega-tivedirections,thusshowingthatdopaminecausallymediatesmusicalrewardexperience.
Authorcontributions:L.
F.
,E.
M.
-H.
,R.
J.
Z.
,M.
V.
,J.
R.
,andA.
R.
-F.
designedresearch;R.
M.
A.
providedlogisticalsupport;L.
F.
,E.
M.
-H.
,A.
G.
-A.
,H.
A.
,G.
O.
,andM.
V.
performedre-search;L.
F.
,E.
M.
-H.
,andG.
O.
analyzeddata;andL.
F.
,E.
M.
-H.
,R.
J.
Z.
,P.
R.
,J.
M.
-P.
,M.
V.
,J.
R.
,andA.
R.
-F.
wrotethepaper.
Theauthorsdeclarenoconflictofinterest.
ThisarticleisaPNASDirectSubmission.
PublishedunderthePNASlicense.
SeeCommentaryonpage3364.
1L.
F.
andE.
M.
-H.
contributedequallytothiswork.
2Towhomcorrespondencemaybeaddressed.
Email:laura.
ferreri@univ-lyon2.
frorantoni.
rodriguez@icrea.
cat.
3M.
V.
,J.
R.
,andA.
R.
-F.
contributedequallytothiswork.
Thisarticlecontainssupportinginformationonlineatwww.
pnas.
org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.
1073/pnas.
1811878116/-/DCSupplemental.
PublishedonlineJanuary22,2019.
www.
pnas.
org/cgi/doi/10.
1073/pnas.
1811878116PNAS|February26,2019|vol.
116|no.
9|3793–3798NEUROSCIENCEPSYCHOLOGICALANDCOGNITIVESCIENCESSEECOMMENTARYDownloadedbyguestonDecember4,2020observedinpriorstudiesisactuallycausing/facilitatingthepleasureweexperiencefrommusicor,incontrast,itisaconse-quenceofthatpleasure,engagingdopamine-relatedlearningandmotivationalsystemsasithasbeenshowninanimalstudiesusingprimaryrewards.
Furthermore,certainauthorshavedistinguishedbetweenvariouskindsofpleasures,rangingfrommoresensory-based,mastery-competencerelated,tomoreaesthetic(refs.
25–27;seeref.
28forarecentdiscussion).
Althoughdopaminemightnotbedirectlyinvolvedinmoresensorypleasurableexperiences,itcouldhoweverintertwinedifferentiallyorinamorecomplexwayintheprocessingofdiversetypesofpleasures,ascouldbethecaseinaestheticexperiences(29).
Directlymanipulatingdopa-minergictransmissionwouldthencriticallyshedlightontheneurobiologyandneurochemistryunderpinningrewardresponsestomusic.
Morebroadly,thisapproachwouldalsodisentanglethecausalroleofthedopaminergicsystem,characterizedbyimpor-tantdifferencesacrossspecies(30,31),inthepleasurablere-sponsesassociatedwithabstractrewardsinhumans.
Weaddressedthisquestionthroughadoubleblindwithin-subjectpharmacologicaldesigninwhichwedirectlymanipulateddopaminergicsynapticavailabilitywhilehealthyparticipantslis-tenedtoself-selectedandexperimenter-selectedmusicalexcerpts(24).
Therefore,weorallyadministratedtoeachparticipantado-pamineprecursor(levodopa),adopamineantagonist(risperidone),andaplacebo(lactose)inthreedifferentsessions(separatedbyatleast1wk).
Thedopamineprecursorlevodopadoesnotin-discriminatelyandmassivelyenhancetonicdopaminelevelsasotherdopamine-enhancingdrugsdo—likemethylphenidateord-amphetamine.
Incontrast,levodopaisrapidlytakenupbydopaminergicneurons,tobetransformedintodopamineandstoredinvesicles,enhancingsynapticdopaminelevelsinassociationwithstimulus-elicitedresponses.
RisperidoneisadopaminergicantagonistthatinterfereswithdopaminergicneurotransmissionbybindingtoaseriesofdopaminereceptorsknownasD2-likereceptors(32).
Wemeasuredpleasureresponsesthrough(i)aphysiologicalmeasureofarousal,electrodermalactivity(EDA),whichisagoodobjectiveindicatorofthehedonicimpactofmusic(19,33);and(ii)subjectiveratingsoftheexperiencedpleasure(real-timeratingsandgeneralpleasureratingsprovidedaftereachsong).
Motivationalresponsesweremeasuredbyaskingparticipantshowmuchoftheirownmoneytheywerewillingtospendforeachsong,usingapreviouslyvalidatedauctionparadigm(12,24).
Tocontrolforotherpossibledopaminergic-dependentmodulations,participantswererequestedtoprovide,foreachexcerpt,subjectiveratingsofemotionalvalence,arousal,andfa-miliarity.
Crucially,tocontrolfortheactualimplicationofrewardprocesses,wealsoemployedanonmusiccondition,themonetaryincentivedelay(MID)paradigm,awell-establishedandexten-sivelyusedprotocolabletoactivatethedopaminergicsystem,whichwasthefocusofstudyhere(19,34,35).
Wepredictedthatifdopamine,beyonditsroleinlearningandmotivation,playsacausalroleinmusic-evokedpleasure,levodopaandrisperidoneadministrationshouldleadtooppositeeffectsinmeasuresofbothmusicalpleasureandmotivation:whilethedopamineprecursorlevodopashouldincreasethehedonicexperienceandthemusic-relatedmotivationalresponses,risperidoneshouldleadtoareductionofboth.
Incontrast,ifdopamineonlyplaysaroleinmotivationaspreviouslydescribedinprimaryrewards,thepharmacologicalinterventionshouldleavepleasurereactionsintactandjustmodulatemusic-relatedmotivationalresponses.
ResultsListeningtopleasurablemusicisoftenaccompaniedbymea-surablebodilyreactionssuchasgoosebumpsorshiversdownthespine,commonlycalled"chills"or"frissons.
"Chillsaregenerallyusedasindicatorsofmusicalpleasure,althoughnoteverybodyexperienceschillsinresponsetomusic.
Inthecurrentexperi-ment,16(outof27)participantsreportedchillsduringtheplacebosessionwitheithertheirownfavoritemusicorourmusicselection.
Weexaminedthetimethesespecificparticipantsreportedchillsacrosssessionstoassesstheeffectsofthephar-macologicalinterventionsonchillresponses.
Tonormalizethesevaluesacrossparticipants(24),wecomputedchangeswithre-specttotheplacebointheamountoftimetheseparticipantsreportedchillsduringmusiclistening(i.
e.
,self-selectedandexperimenter-selectedexcerpts),followingbothlevodopaandrisperidoneadministration(butseeSIAppendix,Figs.
S1–S3forcompleterepresentationofthethreepharmacologicalsessionsandSIAppendix,Fig.
S4fordistributionofindividualresponsestodrugs).
AWilcoxonsigned-ranktestindicatedthatchillre-spondersspentmoretimereportingchillsfollowinglevodopathanrisperidone(Z=2.
341,P0.
173,Wilcoxonsigned-ranktests,seealsoSIAppendix,Fig.
S5).
Pharmacologicalinterventions(i.
e.
,drugs)bidirectionallyaffected(i.
e.
,higherpleasureunderpre-cursor,lowerunderantagonist)bothtypesofexcerptsinthesameway.
Importantly,thiseffectwasnotduetoageneralre-ductionofratingsunderrisperidone,asnosignificantdifferencesintotaltimereportingreal-timeratingswerefoundwhencom-paringthetwodrugs(Z=0.
952,P=0.
341,Wilcoxonsigned-ranktest).
Thesedrugeffectsdidnotdifferbetweenmenandwomen(allP>0.
706,Kruskal–WallisHtests).
WethenevaluatedchangesacrosssessionsinEDAassociatedwithreal-timeratingsofpleasureprovidedbytheparticipants.
PreviousstudieshaveshownthatlisteningtopleasurablemusicisgenerallyaccompaniedbyincreasesofEDAwhicharemodu-latedbytheintensityoftheexperiencedpleasure.
Thus,EDAassociatedwithhighpleasurablemusicalexcerptshasbeengenerallyusedasanobjectivemeasureofmusicalpleasure.
Therefore,wecomputedphysiologicalvaluesassociatedwithhigh-pleasuremusiclisteningperiodsbysubtractingchangesinEDAfollowinglow(real-timeratings1and2)tohighpleas-antnessratings(i.
e.
,refs.
3and4).
Placebo-correctedEDAval-uesassociatedwithhigh-pleasurestatesduringmusiclisteningshowedanincreaseunderlevodopa,significantlydifferentfromthedecreaseobservedunderrisperidone[effectofdrug(levodopa/risperidone),t(25)=2.
261,P=0.
033,pairedttest](Figs.
1Band2A).
NoEDAmodulationswereobservedduringabaselinerestperiod(beforethemusiclisteningtask).
Importantly,theseresultsindicatethatpharmacologicalinterventionsdidnotin-ducechangesinEDAitselfnorgenerallyinEDAresponsetomusic,butrather,affectedthesignalassociatedwithhighhe-donicresponsestomusic.
Inaddition,wealsoanalyzedtheav-erageEDAwhilelisteningtomusic(thusaveragingthe45sofmusiclisteningforallsongslistenedto)oneachsession.
WedidnotfinddifferencesinEDAamplitudefortheentiredurationofthesongsfollowinglevodopaorrisperidone[t(25)=0.
873;P=0.
391,pairedttest].
ThisresultindicatesthatthedopaminergicmanipulationdidnotleadtotonicchangesinEDAwhilelisteningtomusic,butratherspecificallymodulatedtheamplitudeofphasicEDAinresponsetohighlypleasantsectionsofmusic.
Forthecontrolmonetaryrewardtask(MID),andsimilarlytomusiclis-tening,adrug-dependentmodulationeffectwasobservedforhighincentivecondition.
EDAactivityassociatedwithhighmonetarygainsresultedinincreasedanddecreasedconditionsunder3794|www.
pnas.
org/cgi/doi/10.
1073/pnas.
1811878116Ferrerietal.
DownloadedbyguestonDecember4,2020levodopaandrisperidone,respectively[t(26)=2.
868,P=0.
008,pairedttest](Fig.
2B),whilenosignificantdifferenceswereob-servedduringtheneutral(i.
e.
,norewarding)condition[t(26)=0.
926,P=0.
363),pairedttest].
Next,wetestedwhetherdopaminergicmanipulationinflu-encedparticipants'aestheticjudgmentsprovidedaftereachsong(from1=nopleasantnessto5=intensepleasantness).
Fol-lowingthesamerationaleaswiththepreviousanalyses,wecomputedpercentofchangefollowingrisperidoneorlevodopaadministrationwithrespecttoplacebo.
Theanalysisrevealedamarginaldifferencebetweendrugs:individualstendtoreporthigherlikingratesfollowinglevodopathanrisperidone(Z=1.
947,P=0.
052,Wilcoxonsigned-ranktest).
Nosignificantchangeswereobservedbetweendrugsforothersubjectiverat-ingsprovidedaftereachsong,i.
e.
,familiarity,arousal,andemotionalvalence(allP>0.
105Wilcoxonsigned-ranktests).
Finally,andimportantlyforourhypothesis,thepharmaco-logicalinterventiondidmodulateparticipants'willingnesstopayfortheexperimenter-selectedmusic:individualssignificantlybidmoremoneyunderlevodopathanunderrisperidone(Z=2.
435,P=0.
015,Wilcoxonsigned-ranktest).
Aswasthecaseforpleasureresponses,motivationalrewardresponses(monetarybids)increasedunderdopaminergicstimulationanddecreasedunderdopaminergicinhibitionrelativetobaseline(Fig.
1C).
Inlinewithresultsinreal-timeratings,thegeneraldrugeffectonmotivationalresponsesdidnotdifferbetweenmenandwomen[χ2(1)=0.
462,P=0.
497,Kruskal–WallisHtest].
DiscussionOverall,ourresultsstraightforwardlyrevealedthatpharmacologi-calinterventionsbidirectionallymodulatedtherewardresponseselicitedbymusic.
Inparticular,wefoundthatrisperidoneimpairedparticipants'abilitytoexperiencemusicalpleasure,whereaslevo-dopaenhancedit.
Accordingly,emotionalarousalcorrespondingtohigh-pleasurereal-timeratings,asindexedbychangesinEDA,washigherunderthedopamineprecursorandlowerunderthedopa-mineantagonistcomparedwithplacebo.
ThesefindingsparallelthoseobservedinthecontrolMIDtask,whereEDAincreasedunderlevodopaanddecreasedunderrisperidoneinresponsetohigh-rewarding,butnottoneutralnonrewardingmonetarycues.
Finally,participantswerewillingtospendmoremoneyunderlevodopathanunderrisperidone,indicatingthattheyweremoremotivatedtolistentothemusicagainwhenpromotingthedopa-minergictransmissionthanwhenblockingit.
PreviousstudieshaveconsistentlyshowntheimplicationofthemesolimbicrewardsystemforbothhedonicandmotivationalresponsesusingPET(5,6,11,35),fMRI(7–17),andtranscranialmagneticstimulation(TMS)(24).
However,exceptforref.
11,noneofthesestudieswerespecifictodopaminetransmission,andallwerecorrelationalexceptforref.
24,whichhoweverattemptedtoindirectlymodulatedopaminebymeansofTMS.
Here,weprovidecausalevidencefortheimplicationofthedopaminergicsysteminmusicalrewardbydirectlyalteringdo-paminergicfunction.
Severalfindingssupportthatourpharma-cologicalmodulationsspecificallyaffectedrewardresponses,ratherthanamoregeneralmodulationofparticipants'arousalorwell-being.
First,resultsfromthecontrolMIDtask,previouslyrelatedtospecificbrainactivityanddopaminereleaseintheNAcc(34,36),confirmapharmacological-dependentmodula-tionoftherewardsystem.
Iftheinterventionwasaffectingdopamine-dependentrewardresponses,changesshouldbepre-sentinadopamine-dependenttasksuchastheMID.
Adminis-trationofdopamineprecursorwasindeedassociatedwithanincreaseinEDAforhighcomparedwithlowmagnitudeofmonetaryreward-predictingcues,andthisresponsedecreasedunderrisperidone.
Inaddition,neutraltrials,inwhichpartici-pantsdidnotloseorwinmoney,wereusedasanegativecontroltoascertainwhetherpharmacologicalmodulationwasconfinedtoreward-relatedprocesses,andthusruleoutunspecificdrugeffectsonbaselinedopaminergictone.
Crucially,nodifferencesacrossconditions(i.
e.
,drugs)werefoundfortheneutralcondi-tion,indicatinganinteractionbetweenrewardprocessingspe-cificallyanddopaminergicmodulation.
Second,EDAvaluesweresimilarlymodulatedinresponsetohigh-comparedwithlow-rewardingmusicalexcerpts,butnodifferencesacrossses-sionswerefoundinthebaselinecondition.
Finally,atthebe-haviorallevel,onlythereward-relatedsubjectiveratingsweremodulatedbypharmacologicalinterventions,whilenosignificantchangesinemotionalvalence,arousal,andfamiliarityratingswerefoundacrosssessions.
Thisisinlinewithrecentfindingsonwordlearning[sameinterventions,sameindividuals(37);par-ticipantscompletedthewordlearningtaskafterourmusicpara-digm]showingthatthebehavioralratingsassociatedwithrewardduringlearning,butnottheonesrelatedtoaneutralconditionortoarousal,wereaffectedbythepharmacologicalinterventionsinthesamemannerastheonesreportedhere.
Altogether,thesepointsofferconvergingevidencethatourfindingspointedlyreflectadrug-dependentmodulationofrewardexperienceandexcludeanunspecificandconfoundinggeneraldrugeffectaf-fectingparticipants'healthormoodstate.
Inthisvein,BerridgeandKringelbach(23)describethreeinterconnectedbutneurobiologicallydifferentpsychologicalcomponentsofreward:"liking,"namelythehedonicimpact,plea-surablecomponent;"wanting,"namelythemotivationforre-ward;and"learning,"namelytheassociations,representations,Fig.
1.
Changeswithrespecttoplaceboconditionunderlevodopaandrisperidone(means±SEM)formusic-relatedrewardresponses.
(A)Amountoftimereportingchills,highpleasure(HP),lowpleasure(LP),andnoplea-sure(NP)real-timeratings(i.
e.
,whilelisteningtoeachsong,includingself-andexperimenter-selectedsongs;e.
g.
,risperidoneandlevodopa,respectively,decreasedandincreasedby43%and65%thetimereportingchillswithrespecttotheplacebocondition).
Chillsvaluesincludechillsrespondersonly;HP,LP,andNPvaluesincludetheentiresample.
(B)EDAchangesassociatedwithhigh-pleasurereal-timeratings.
(C)Motivationalratings,i.
e.
,willingnesstospendmoneyforeachexcerpt.
*P<0.
05.
Fig.
2.
EDAchanges(means,solidlines±SEM,lightercolors)overtimewithrespecttoplaceboconditionunderlevodopa(green)andrisperidone(red)duringmusiclisteningassociatedwithhigh-rewardingexcerpts(i.
e.
,highminuslowreal-timelikingrates)(A)showsverysimilarresponsestoEDAchangesassociatedwithhigh-rewardingcues(i.
e.
,highmonetarygainminuslowmonetarygain)intheMIDcontroltask(B).
Ferrerietal.
PNAS|February26,2019|vol.
116|no.
9|3795NEUROSCIENCEPSYCHOLOGICALANDCOGNITIVESCIENCESSEECOMMENTARYDownloadedbyguestonDecember4,2020andpredictionsaboutfuturerewardsbasedonpastexperiences.
Ifontheonehandtheroleofdopamineinwantingandlearningcomponentsiswidelyaccepted,ontheotherhand,itsfunctioninregulatinghedonicresponsesappearscontroversial.
Forexample,nearcompletedestructionofnigrostriatalandmesolimbicdopa-minergicneuronsledtonochangesinpleasurealthoughitwasabletocompletelyabolishrats'interestinfood(38).
Nevertheless,thestudiesinvestigatingrewardprocessingusuallydealwithpri-maryrewards,suchasfood,andanimalmodels(e.
g.
,refs.
39–41).
Here,incontrast,studyingresponsestoabstractrewardsinhumansubjects,weshowthatmanipulationofdopaminergictransmis-sionaffectsboththepleasure(i.
e.
,amountoftimereportingchillsandemotionalarousalmeasuredbyEDA)andthemotivationalcomponentsofmusicalreward(moneywillingtospend).
Thesefindingssuggestthatdopaminergicsignalingisasinequanonconditionnotonlyformotivationalresponses,ashasbeenshownwithprimaryandsecondaryrewards,butalsoforhedonicreac-tionstomusic.
Thisresultsupportsrecentfindingsshowingthatdopaminealsomediatestheperceivedpleasantnessattainedbyothertypesofabstractrewards(37)andchallengespreviousfindingsinanimalmodelsonprimaryrewards,suchasfood(42,43).
Musicalpleasure,incontrasttowhatoccurswithprimaryre-wards,maydependonmodulationsofemotionalarousalandintensity(6)triggeredbyexpectationsandsurprisesdrivenbythepresenceofstructuralandtemporalregularitiesinmusicalpat-terns(12,44,45),associativeconditioning(29,46),orepisodicmemory(47)amongothers(48–50).
Importantly,mostoftheseelementsrelyoncognitivecomputationsdrivenbydopaminergictransmission:fromdopamine'sroleinlearningtoitsroleinmemoryorattention(seeref.
31).
Inthisregard,levodopahasbeenshowntoenhancecognitivefunctionespeciallyinmemoryandlearning(51–53),butalsoinotherdomains,suchassemanticactivationandpriming(54–56),andevenfeedback-basedgrammarlearning(57).
Onthecontrary,theblockadeofD2receptorsmayleadtocognitiveimpairments(58),especiallyinexecutivefunctionsandmemory(59,60).
Importantly,drugintakeonlyinfluencedreward-relatedratingssuchasreal-timeratingsofpleasure,physiologicalresponsetopleasure,andparticipants'willingnesstobuyourmusicselection,butitdidnotdistortotherfunctionsimportantformusicperception.
Drugintakedidnotaltertheirabilitytorecognizefamiliarmelodies—indicatingthatthedrugsdidnotinterferewiththerecallandrecognitionofpreviousmusicexperiences—anditdidnotmodulateperceivedemotionalvalenceorgeneralarousal,whichmayberelatedtoconcreteacousticattributessuchaspitch,mode,tempo,orloudness(49,50).
Thus,onepotentialinterpretationofourresults,togetherwithpreviousevidenceinataskinvolvingimplicitrewardthroughsuccessfullearning(37),isthatdopamine-dependentcognitiveprocessessuchaslearningandmemory—modulatedupanddownbylevodopaandrisperidone,respectively—maysub-stantiallyinfluencethepleasuregeneratedbyabstractactivities.
Thisviewdoesnotnecessarilyimplythatdopaminergictransmissiongeneratesthehedonicexperienceperse.
Itcouldbegeneratedbyotherneurotransmittercircuitriesthatinteractwiththedopaminesystem.
Previousanimalstudieswithprimaryrewardshaveshowntheexistenceofso-called"hedonichotspots"inthebrainthatareresponsibleforthegenerationofpleasure(61).
Thesehedonichotspots,foundalongtherewardcircuitryintheNAcc,insula,orbitofrontalcortex,andventralpallidum,aremodulatedbyopioidtransmission(62).
Bothopioidanddopa-minesystemsarecolocalizedandtheyinteractincomplexways.
Whileopioidmodulationofdopamineneuronshasbeenexten-sivelystudiedduetoitsrelevanceinaddiction(63–66),afewstudieshaveexploreddopaminemodulationofendogenousopi-oidpeptidereleases(67–69).
Interestingly,dopamine-stimulatingdrugs,actingviaD2dopaminereceptors,cancausethereleaseofendogenousopioidpeptideswithintheNAcc(69).
Thus,whiledopaminergicfunctionmaybeimportantandanindispensablestepformusicalpleasure,theultimatesystemresponsiblemightbetheopioidcircuit,asoccursinprimaryrewards.
However,theexactmechanismsandconsequencesofthisinteractionarestillunknownanditisuncleartowhatextentdopaminergicmodulationofopioidreleasemayaffecthedonichotspotsinparticular[representingaround10%oftheNAccneurons(62)].
Inthisregard,Malliketal.
(70),haverecentlyshownthatpharmacologicalreductionofopioidtransmissionvianaltrexoneledtoageneralreductionofpleasur-ableresponsestobothpleasantandnonpleasantmusic.
However,thefactthatthemanipulationalsoaffectedpleasureresponsesinnonpleasantmusic—inwhichnoopioidreleasewouldhavebeenconsequentlyexpected—doesnotallowtherulingoutofunspecificdrugeffects.
FurtherpharmacologicalinterventionsandPETstudiestargetingopioidanddopaminetransmissionarewarrantedtobetterunderstandthecomplexinterplaybetweenthesetwoneurotransmittersystemsinmusicalpleasure.
Conversely,analternativeexplanationofourresultsisthatthefeelingsofpleasureevokedbyabstractrewards,andmusicinparticular,maydifferfromthoseevokedbyprimaryrewardsandthusdrivenbydifferentpsychologicalandneurobiologicalsub-strates(refs.
25–27,butseeref.
28).
Indeed,theexistenceofdiversetypesofpleasurableexperiencesinhumansremainscurrentlyanopendebate(25–27).
Aspreviouslymentioned,music-evokedpleasureisdriven,amongotherthings,byitsintrinsicabilitytoinducefeelingssuchasanticipationandeuphoria.
Remarkably,previousstudieshaveshownthatdopamine-stimulatingdrugssuchascocaineoramphetamineelicitsimilarpositiveaffectivestatesinhumans(71).
DirectelectricalbrainstimulationoftheNAcchasbeenshowntoelicitsmilesandeuphoria(72).
Similarly,microin-jectionsofpsychostimulantsintotheNAccelicit50-kHzultrasonicvocalizationsinrats,proposedtoreflectapositiveappetitiveaf-fectivestate(73).
Althoughthesebehaviorshavebeenrelatedtothemotivationalaspectsofreward,onemaywonderifthesefeel-ingsmaybeinterpretedaspleasurable.
Inthisregard,dopaminereleasesdrivenbymusicmightincreasetheattractivenessofthesurroundingenvironmentandthemotivationtopursueanddesiresimilarexperiencesleadingtopositivefeelingsthatareattributedto"pleasurable"(12,21).
Thus,subjectiveexperienceofmusicalpleasure,similartoemotions(74),mayarisefrommotivationalsignalsandcognitiveappraisalratherthanthroughtheengagementofthehedonichotspotspreviouslyidentifiedwithprimaryrewards.
Inconclusion,thepresentfindingsshowacausalroleofdo-pamineinmusicalpleasureandshedlightontheroleofthehumandopaminergicsysteminabstractrewards.
Morebroadly,theseresultsindicatethatdopaminergictransmissionmightplaydifferentoradditiverolesthantheonespostulatedinaffectiveprocessingsofar,particularlyinabstractcognitiveactivities.
MaterialandMethodsParticipants.
ThestudywascarriedoutinaccordancewiththeDeclarationofHelsinkiandtheInternationalConferenceonHarmonization'sGoodClinicalPracticeGuidelines.
Allvolunteersgavetheirwritteninformedconsentforparticipationbeforeanyprocedure.
Twenty-ninevolunteers(seeref.
37andSIAppendixforprescreeningandselectioncriteria)wererandomizedandcompletedthestudy(19females,meanage=22.
83±4.
39)inexchangeformonetarycompensationaccordingtoSpanishlegislation.
Theoriginalsamplesizewaschosentobe30participants,but1participantdroppedoutearlyinthestudyandonly29finishedit.
SelectedparticipantswerealsotestedwiththeextendedversionoftheBarcelonamusicrewardquestionnaire(BMRQ)(ref.
19,seealsoref.
75).
Noparticipantspresentedsignsofamusia.
Twoparticipantsscoredwithintherangesconsideredtoindicatemusicalanhedo-nia(BMRQ=61and64)andgeneralanhedonia(physicalanhedoniascale=19and24),andwerethereforeexcludedfromtheanalysisherereported(totaln=27,18females,meanage=23±4.
48,meanBMRQ=77.
07±9.
89).
Procedure.
Thisdouble-blind,crossover,treatmentsequence-randomizedstudy(SIAppendix,TableS3)wasperformedattheNeuropsychopharmacologyUnitandCenterforDrugResearchoftheSantaCreuiSantPauHospitalof3796|www.
pnas.
org/cgi/doi/10.
1073/pnas.
1811878116Ferrerietal.
DownloadedbyguestonDecember4,2020Barcelona.
Experimentaltestingtookplaceoverthreesessions.
ThestudywasapprovedbytheethicscommitteeoftheHospitaldelaSantaCreuiSantPauandtheSpanishMedicinesandMedicalDevicesAgency(EudraCT2016–000801-35).
Foreachsession,participantsarrivedatthehospitalunderfastingconditionsandweregivenalightbreakfast.
Subsequently,theyreceivedinadouble-blindmaskedfashionacapsulecontainingthetreatment(seeSIAp-pendixformoredetailedinformationaboutdrugdoses):adopaminergicprecursorwithaninhibitorofperipheraldopaminemetabolism(levodopa,100mg+carbidopa,25mg),adopaminereceptorantagonist(risperidone,2mg),oraplacebo(lactose).
Onehourafterdrugadministration,theexper-imentalsessionstarted.
After3minofbaseline(silence,relaxed,openeyes),themusicrewardtasktookplace.
Thesessionthencontinuedwithothertasks(musicalmemoryandlanguagelearning,notreportedinthisarticle)andendedwiththecontroltask,theMID.
Thetotaldurationofthemusicrewardtaskwasabout15min.
ThetotaldurationoftheMIDtaskwasabout20min.
Thetotaldurationoftheexperimentalsessionwasabout2.
5h.
Next,partic-ipantsspenttheirtimeinarestingroomandwereallowedtoleavethehospitalafter6hfromthetreatmentadministration.
Atleast1wkpassedbetweenonesessionandthenext.
MusicTask.
Participantswererequestedtolistentothe10popsongsandtotheirfivefavoritemusicalexcerptsfollowingasimilarproceduretothatinref.
24.
Theorderofpresentationofmusicalexcerptswithineachgroupofsongswasrandomizedacrossparticipants.
Beforestartingwitheachsession,atrialpopsong("Sinsaberporqué"byVanessaMartín;neitherthesongnortheperformerwaspartofthemusicselectedfortheexperiment)wasplayedtomakesurethateachparticipantunderstoodthetaskandthesubjectiveratingstoprovide.
Participantswereseatedinacomfortablechairinfrontofascreen.
Eachexcerpt(seeSIAppendixfordetailsaboutmusicalstimuli)waspresentedviaheadphoneswhileamusicalnote(whitesymbolonblackbackground)appearedonthescreen.
Duringeachexcerpt,participantswereaskedtoindicateinrealtimethedegreeofpleasuretheywereexperiencingwhilelisteningtothemusic.
Participantswereinstructedtopressoneoffouravailablebuttonsonakeyboard(1=nopleasure,2=lowpleasure,3=highpleasure,4=chills)dependingonthepleasuretheywereexperiencing.
Theywereinstructedtochangetheratingbypressinganotherbuttonassoonastheyfeltthattheirexperiencedpleasurewaschanging.
Thesameprocedurehasbeenextensivelyusedinpreviousstudiesonmusicalrewardprocessing(11,17,19,24,33).
Similarly,aftereachexcerpt,participantsprovidedad-ditionalratingsonthatsongbyansweringquestionsappearingonthescreen.
Inparticular,theywereaskedtoratethegeneralpleasantness(from1=nogeneralpleasantnessto5=intensegeneralpleasantness),arousal(from1=veryrelaxingto5=veryarousing),andemotionalvalence(from1=verysadto5=veryhappy)theyfeltinresponsetoeachexcerpt.
Forpopmusic,participantswereadditionallyaskedtoratethefamiliarityofeachsong(from1=completelyunfamiliarto5=IhavethesonginmyPC,mp3,Spotifyplaylist,etc).
Rewardratings(i.
e.
,real-timeandpleasantnessratings)maybeconsideredmeasuresof"liking"processes.
Tohavea"wanting"motivationalrating,weintroducedanauctionparadigminwhichpartici-pantscouldpurchasethepopmusicwiththeirownmoney,asanindicationthattheywantedtohearitagain(adaptedfromrefs.
12and24).
Foreachexperimenter-selectedsong,participantscouldindicatewhethertheywerewillingtopayV0,V0.
29,V0.
99,orV1.
29(seerefs.
12and24andSIAp-pendixformoredetailedinformationabouttheauctionparadigm).
MIDTask.
TheMIDtaskconsistedof35trials.
Atthebeginningofatrial,participantssawoneoffivecueshapes(2s),signalingwhetherparticipantswereplayingtowinpotentialrewards(14trials;denotedbycircles)ortoavoidlosingpotentiallosses(14trials;denotedbysquares).
Themagnitudeofthepossibleoutcomeswasindicatedusinghorizontallinesinthecue,andcouldbelarge(1V,threehorizontallines,seventrialsforeachvalence)orsmall(0.
1V,onehorizontalline,seventrialsforeachvalence).
Sixsecondsaftercueoffset,participantshadtorespond,asfastaspossible,withabuttonpresstoawhitetargetsquarethatappearedforavariablelengthoftime(target,160–260ms).
Inwintrials,ifparticipantsrespondedontimetheyobtainedthecorrespondingamountofmoney.
Incontrast,inlosstrials,ifparticipantsrespondedontimetheyavoidedlosingthecorrespondingamountofmoney.
Sixsecondsafterparticipants'response,avisualfeedbackonthescreennotifiedwhethertheyhadwonorlostmoneyduringthattrial.
Eightsecondslater,anothercuewaspresented.
Additionally,aneutralcondition(seventrials;denotedbyatriangle)inwhichparticipantswerenotplayingformoneywasalsoincluded.
Taskdifficulty,basedonreactiontimescollectedduringthepracticesession,wassetsuchthateachparticipantcouldsucceedon66%ofhis/hertargetresponses.
Trialtypeswererandomlyorderedwithineachsession.
AnalysisofBehavioralRatingsandEDA.
Thepercentageofchangeunderrisperidoneandlevodopawithrespecttoplacebowascomputedforeachmeasure.
Placebo-correctedvaluesofsubjectiveratings(i.
e.
,realtimeandratingsprovidedaftereachsong)betweenpharmacologicalsessions,andgenderdifferencesingeneraldrugeffects(SIAppendix,Figs.
S4andS5)werecomparedusingnonparametrictests(i.
e.
,Wilcoxonsigned-rankandKruskal–WallisHtests,asfullydescribedinSIAppendix).
EDAwasrecordedfollowingtheproceduresdescribedinref.
17andanalyzedbycomputingtheproportionofamplitudechangescorrespondingtohigh-andlow-pleasureresponses,forbothmusicandMIDtasks.
Placebo-correctednormalizedvalueswerecom-paredbetweenpharmacologicalsessionsusingpaired-samplettests(allde-tailedinformationreportedinSIAppendix).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS.
WethankthestaffoftheCentred'InvestigaciódelMedicamentdel'InstitutdeRecercadel'HospitaldelaSantaCreuiSantPau(HSCSP)fortheirhelp.
ThepresentprojecthasbeenfundedbytheSpanishGovernment(MINECOGrantPSI2011-29219toA.
R.
-F.
andAP2010-4179toP.
R.
).
L.
F.
waspartiallysupportedbyaMorelli-Rotarypostdoctoralfellow-ship.
M.
V.
waspartiallysupportedbyFondodeInvestigaciónenSaludthroughGrantCP04/00121fromtheSpanishHealthMinistryincollabora-tionwithl'InstitutdeRecercadeHSCSP;sheisamemberofCentrodeInvestigaciónBiomédicaenRedSaludMental(fundedbytheSpanishHealthMinistry,InstitutodeSaludCarlosIII).
H.
A.
wassupportedbyagrantfromtheSpanishGovernment(BES-2013-067440).
1.
DubéL,LeBelJ(2003)Thecontentandstructureoflaypeople'sconceptofpleasure.
CognEmotion17:263–295.
2.
BigandE,Poulin-CharronnatB(2006)Arewe"experiencedlisteners"Areviewofthemusicalcapacitiesthatdonotdependonformalmusicaltraining.
Cognition100:100–130.
3.
PatelAD(2008)Science&music:Talkofthetone.
Nature453:726–727.
4.
LaceyS,etal.
(2011)Artforreward'ssake:Visualartrecruitstheventralstriatum.
Neuroimage55:420–433.
5.
BloodAJ,ZatorreRJ,BermudezP,EvansAC(1999)Emotionalresponsestopleasantandunpleasantmusiccorrelatewithactivityinparalimbicbrainregions.
NatNeurosci2:382–387.
6.
BloodAJ,ZatorreRJ(2001)Intenselypleasurableresponsestomusiccorrelatewithactivityinbrainregionsimplicatedinrewardandemotion.
ProcNatlAcadSciUSA98:11818–11823.
7.
KoelschS(2005)Investigatingemotionwithmusic:Neuroscientificapproaches.
AnnNYAcadSci1060:412–418.
8.
BernsGS,CapraCM,MooreS,NoussairC(2010)Neuralmechanismsoftheinfluenceofpopularityonadolescentratingsofmusic.
Neuroimage49:2687–2696.
9.
MontagC,ReuterM,AxmacherN(2011)Howone'sfavoritesongactivatesthere-wardcircuitryofthebrain:Personalitymatters!
BehavBrainRes225:511–514.
10.
PereiraCS,etal.
(2011)Musicandemotionsinthebrain:Familiaritymatters.
PLoSOne6:e27241.
11.
SalimpoorVN,BenovoyM,LarcherK,DagherA,ZatorreRJ(2011)Anatomicallydis-tinctdopaminereleaseduringanticipationandexperienceofpeakemotiontomusic.
NatNeurosci14:257–262.
12.
SalimpoorVN,etal.
(2013)Interactionsbetweenthenucleusaccumbensandauditorycorticespredictmusicrewardvalue.
Science340:216–219.
13.
KoelschS(2014)Braincorrelatesofmusic-evokedemotions.
NatRevNeurosci15:170–180.
14.
FrühholzS,TrostW,GrandjeanD(2014)Theroleofthemedialtemporallimbicsysteminprocessingemotionsinvoiceandmusic.
ProgNeurobiol123:1–17.
15.
MuellerK,etal.
(2015)Investigatingthedynamicsofthebrainresponsetomusic:Acentralroleoftheventralstriatum/nucleusaccumbens.
Neuroimage116:68–79.
16.
BratticoE,etal.
(2016)It'ssadbutIlikeit:Theneuraldissociationbetweenmusicalemotionsandlikinginexpertsandlaypersons.
FrontHumNeurosci9:676.
17.
Martínez-MolinaN,Mas-HerreroE,Rodríguez-FornellsA,ZatorreRJ,Marco-PallarésJ(2016)Neuralcorrelatesofspecificmusicalanhedonia.
ProcNatlAcadSciUSA113:E7337–E7345.
18.
Mas-HerreroE,Marco-PallaresJ,Lorenzo-SevaU,ZatorreRJ,Rodriguez-FornellsA(2013)Individualdifferencesinmusicrewardexperiences.
MusicPercept31:118–138.
19.
Mas-HerreroE,ZatorreRJ,Rodriguez-FornellsA,Marco-PallarésJ(2014)Dissociationbetweenmusicalandmonetaryrewardresponsesinspecificmusicalanhedonia.
CurrBiol24:699–704.
20.
ZaldDH,ZatorreRJ(2011)Music.
NeurobiologyofSensationandReward,edGottfriedJA(CRC,BocaRaton,FL),Chap19.
21.
GebauerL,KringelbachML,VuustP(2012)Ever-changingcyclesofmusicalpleasure:Theroleofdopamineandanticipation.
Psychomusicology22:152–167.
22.
ZatorreRJ,SalimpoorVN(2013)Fromperceptiontopleasure:Musicanditsneuralsubstrates.
ProcNatlAcadSciUSA110:10430–10437.
23.
BerridgeKC,KringelbachML(2008)Affectiveneuroscienceofpleasure:Rewardinhumansandanimals.
Psychopharmacology(Berl)199:457–480.
Ferrerietal.
PNAS|February26,2019|vol.
116|no.
9|3797NEUROSCIENCEPSYCHOLOGICALANDCOGNITIVESCIENCESSEECOMMENTARYDownloadedbyguestonDecember4,202024.
Mas-HerreroE,DagherA,ZatorreRJ(2018)Modulatingmusicalrewardsensitivityupanddownwithtranscranialmagneticstimulation.
NatHumBehav2:27–32.
25.
BerlyneDE,MadsenKB(1973)Pleasure,Reward,Preference:TheirNature,Determinants,andRoleinBehaviour,edsBerlyneDE,MadsenKB(Academic,NewYork).
26.
RozinP(1999)Preadaptationandthepuzzlesandpropertiesofpleasure.
Well-being:TheFoundationsofHedonicPsychology,edsKahnemanD,DienerE,SchwarzN(RussellSageFoundation,NewYork),pp109–133.
27.
FrijdaNH(2001)Thenatureofpleasure.
UnravelingtheComplexitiesofSocialLife:AFestschriftinHonorofRobertB.
Zajonc,edsBarghJA,ApsleyDK(APA,Washington,DC),pp71–94.
28.
SkovM,NadalM(2018)Artisnotspecial:Anassaultonthelastlinesofdefenseagainstthenaturalizationofthehumanmind.
RevNeurosci29:699–702.
29.
PankseppJ,BernatzkyG(2002)Emotionalsoundsandthebrain:Theneuro-affectivefoundationsofmusicalappreciation.
BehavProcesses60:133–155.
30.
RaghantiMA,etal.
(2016)Human-specificincreaseofdopaminergicinnervationinastriatalregionassociatedwithspeechandlanguage:Acomparativeanalysisoftheprimatebasalganglia.
JCompNeurol524:2117–2129.
31.
SousaAMM,etal.
(2017)Molecularandcellularreorganizationofneuralcircuitsinthehumanlineage.
Science358:1027–1032.
32.
BursteinES,etal.
(2005)IntrinsicefficacyofantipsychoticsathumanD2,D3,andD4dopaminereceptors:IdentificationoftheclozapinemetaboliteN-desmethylclozapineasaD2/D3partialagonist.
JPharmacolExpTher315:1278–1287.
33.
SalimpoorVN,BenovoyM,LongoG,CooperstockJR,ZatorreRJ(2009)Therewardingaspectsofmusiclisteningarerelatedtodegreeofemotionalarousal.
PLoSOne4:e7487.
34.
SchottBH,etal.
(2008)Mesolimbicfunctionalmagneticresonanceimagingactiva-tionsduringrewardanticipationcorrelatewithreward-relatedventralstriataldo-paminerelease.
JNeurosci28:14311–14319.
35.
BrownS,MartinezMJ,ParsonsLM(2004)Passivemusiclisteningspontaneouslyen-gageslimbicandparalimbicsystems.
Neuroreport15:2033–2037.
36.
KnutsonB,AdamsCM,FongGW,HommerD(2001)Anticipationofincreasingmonetaryrewardselectivelyrecruitsnucleusaccumbens.
JNeurosci21:RC159.
37.
RipollésP,etal.
(2018)Intrinsicallyregulatedlearningismodulatedbysynapticdo-paminesignaling.
eLife7:e38113.
38.
BerridgeKC,RobinsonTE(1998)Whatistheroleofdopamineinreward:Hedonicimpact,rewardlearning,orincentivesalienceBrainResBrainResRev28:309–369.
39.
PeciaS,BerridgeKC(2000)Opioidsiteinnucleusaccumbensshellmediateseatingandhedonic'liking'forfood:MapbasedonmicroinjectionFosplumes.
BrainRes863:71–86.
40.
MahlerSV,SmithKS,BerridgeKC(2007)Endocannabinoidhedonichotspotforsen-sorypleasure:Anandamideinnucleusaccumbensshellenhances'liking'ofasweetreward.
Neuropsychopharmacology32:2267–2278.
41.
WyvellCL,BerridgeKC(2000)Intra-accumbensamphetamineincreasesthecondi-tionedincentivesalienceofsucrosereward:Enhancementofreward"wanting"withoutenhanced"liking"orresponsereinforcement.
JNeurosci20:8122–8130.
42.
BerridgeKC(1996)Foodreward:Brainsubstratesofwantingandliking.
NeurosciBiobehavRev20:1–25.
43.
BerridgeKC,RobinsonTE,AldridgeJW(2009)Dissectingcomponentsofreward:'Liking','wanting',andlearning.
CurrOpinPharmacol9:65–73.
44.
MeyerLB(1956)EmotionandMeaninginMusic(UniversityofChicagoPress,Chicago).
45.
HuronDB(2006)SweetAnticipation:MusicandthePsychologyofExpectation(MITPress,Cambridge,MA).
46.
SachsME,DamasioA,HabibiA(2015)Thepleasuresofsadmusic:Asystematicreview.
FrontHumNeurosci9:404.
47.
JanataP(2009)Theneuralarchitectureofmusic-evokedautobiographicalmemories.
CerebCortex19:2579–2594.
48.
HuronD(2004)Music-engenderedlaughter:AnanalysisofhumordevicesinPDQBach.
Proceedingsofthe8thInternationalConferenceonMusicPerceptionandCognition(CausalProductions,Adelaide,Australia),700–704.
49.
JuslinPN,VstfjllD(2008)Emotionalresponsestomusic:Theneedtoconsiderun-derlyingmechanisms.
BehavBrainSci31:559–575,discussion575–621.
50.
JuslinPN(2013)Fromeverydayemotionstoaestheticemotions:Towardsaunifiedtheoryofmusicalemotions.
PhysLifeRev10:235–266.
51.
KnechtS,etal.
(2004)Levodopa:Fasterandbetterwordlearninginnormalhumans.
AnnNeurol56:20–26.
52.
ChowdhuryR,Guitart-MasipM,BunzeckN,DolanRJ,DüzelE(2012)Dopaminemodulatesepisodicmemorypersistenceinoldage.
JNeurosci32:14193–14204.
53.
ShellshearL,etal.
(2015)Levodopaenhancesexplicitnew-wordlearninginhealthyadults:Apreliminarystudy.
HumPsychopharmacol30:341–349.
54.
AngwinAJ,etal.
(2009)SemanticactivationinParkinson'sdiseasepatientsonandofflevodopa.
Cortex45:950–959.
55.
AngwinAJ,etal.
(2004)Dopamineandsemanticactivation:Aninvestigationofmaskeddirectandindirectpriming.
JIntNeuropsycholSoc10:15–25.
56.
CoplandDA,McMahonKL,SilburnPA,deZubicarayGI(2009)Dopaminergicneuro-modulationofsemanticprocessing:A4-TFMRIstudywithlevodopa.
CerebCortex19:2651–2658.
57.
deVriesMH,UlteC,ZwitserloodP,SzymanskiB,KnechtS(2010)Increasingdopaminelevelsinthebrainimprovesfeedback-basedprocedurallearninginhealthypartici-pants:Anartificial-grammar-learningexperiment.
Neuropsychologia48:3193–3197.
58.
SakuraiH,etal.
(2013)DopamineD2receptoroccupancyandcognitioninschizo-phrenia:AnalysisoftheCATIEdata.
SchizophrBull39:564–574.
59.
HoriH,etal.
(2006)Antipsychoticmedicationandcognitivefunctioninschizophrenia.
SchizophrRes86:138–146.
60.
UchidaH,etal.
(2009)D2receptorblockadebyrisperidonecorrelateswithattentiondeficitsinlate-lifeschizophrenia.
JClinPsychopharmacol29:571–575.
61.
PeciaS,SmithKS,BerridgeKC(2006)Hedonichotspotsinthebrain.
Neuroscientist12:500–511.
62.
BerridgeKC,KringelbachML(2015)Pleasuresystemsinthebrain.
Neuron86:646–664.
63.
MatsuiA,WilliamsJT(2011)Opioid-sensitiveGABAinputsfromrostromedialteg-mentalnucleussynapseontomidbraindopamineneurons.
JNeurosci31:17729–17735.
64.
vanZessenR,PhillipsJL,BudyginEA,StuberGD(2012)ActivationofVTAGABAneuronsdisruptsrewardconsumption.
Neuron73:1184–1194.
65.
HjelmstadGO,XiaY,MargolisEB,FieldsHL(2013)Opioidmodulationofventralpallidalafferentstoventraltegmentalareaneurons.
JNeurosci33:6454–6459.
66.
MargolisEB,HjelmstadGO,FujitaW,FieldsHL(2014)Directbidirectionalμ-opioidcontrolofmidbraindopamineneurons.
JNeurosci34:14707–14716.
67.
Roth-DeriI,etal.
(2003)Effectofexperimenter-deliveredandself-administeredco-caineonextracellularβ-endorphinlevelsinthenucleusaccumbens.
JNeurochem84:930–938.
68.
SodermanAR,UnterwaldEM(2009)Cocaine-inducedmuopioidreceptoroccupancywithinthestriatumismediatedbydopamineD2receptors.
BrainRes1296:63–71.
69.
ColasantiA,etal.
(2012)Endogenousopioidreleaseinthehumanbrainrewardsysteminducedbyacuteamphetamineadministration.
BiolPsychiatry72:371–377.
70.
MallikA,ChandaML,LevitinDJ(2017)Anhedoniatomusicandmu-opioids:Evidencefromtheadministrationofnaltrexone.
SciRep7:41952.
71.
SwansonJM,VolkowND(2003)Serumandbrainconcentrationsofmethylphenidate:Implicationsforuseandabuse.
NeurosciBiobehavRev27:615–621.
72.
OkunMS,etal.
(2004)What'sina"smile"Intra-operativeobservationsofcontra-lateralsmilesinducedbydeepbrainstimulation.
Neurocase10:271–279.
73.
BurgdorfJ,KnutsonB,PankseppJ,IkemotoS(2001)Nucleusaccumbensamphet-aminemicroinjectionsunconditionallyelicit50-kHzultrasonicvocalizationsinrats.
BehavNeurosci115:940–944.
74.
BarrettLF,MesquitaB,OchsnerKN,GrossJJ(2007)Theexperienceofemotion.
AnnuRevPsychol58:373–403.
75.
FerreriL,Rodriguez-FornellsA(2017)Music-relatedrewardresponsespredictepisodicmemoryperformance.
ExpBrainRes235:3721–3731.
3798|www.
pnas.
org/cgi/doi/10.
1073/pnas.
1811878116Ferrerietal.
DownloadedbyguestonDecember4,2020

SugarHosts新增Windows云服务器sugarhosts六折无限流量云服务器六折优惠

SugarHosts糖果主机商我们较早的站长们肯定是熟悉的,早年是提供虚拟主机起家的,如今一直还在提供虚拟主机,后来也有增加云服务器、独立服务器等。数据中心涵盖美国、德国、香港等。我们要知道大部分的海外主机商都只提供Linux系统云服务器。今天,糖果主机有新增SugarHosts夏季六折的优惠,以及新品Windows云服务器/云VPS上线。SugarHosts Windows系统云服务器有区分限制...

Linode 18周年庆典活动 不断改进产品结构和体验

今天早上相比很多网友和一样收到来自Linode的庆祝18周年的邮件信息。和往年一样,他们会回顾在过去一年中的成绩,以及在未来准备改进的地方。虽然目前Linode商家没有提供以前JP1优化线路的机房,但是人家一直跟随自己的脚步在走,确实在云服务器市场上有自己的立足之地。我们看看过去一年中Linode的成就:第一、承诺投入 100,000 美元来帮助具有社会意识的非营利组织,促进有价值的革新。第二、发...

Boomer.host:$4.95/年-512MB/5GB/500GB/德克萨斯州(休斯顿)

部落曾经在去年分享过一次Boomer.host的信息,商家自述始于2018年,提供基于OpenVZ架构的VPS主机,配置不高价格较低。最近,主机商又在LET发了几款特价年付主机促销,最低每年仅4.95美元起,有独立IPv4+IPv6,开设在德克萨斯州休斯顿机房。下面列出几款VPS主机配置信息。CPU:1core内存:512MB硬盘:5G SSD流量:500GB/500Mbps架构:KVMIP/面板...

mid歌曲为你推荐
photoimpact教程PhotoImpact 7 中文版无线路由器限速设置无线路由器限速怎么设置!拂晓雅阁?英特尔和AMD有什么区别啊?显卡温度多少正常显卡温度多少算正常?iphone越狱后怎么恢复苹果越狱后怎么恢复出厂设置硬盘人克隆一个人需要多少人多长时间啊开机滚动条电脑开机滚动条要走好几次bt封杀BT下载被封锁了,怎么办,下载不了电影了!2012年正月十五农历2012年正月15早上9点多生的!命里缺什么!是什么命相如何快速收录如何掌握百度收录之快速收录
欧洲免费vps 美国加州vps 香港机房托管 java空间 亚马逊香港官网 yundun 512mb 空间购买 德隆中文网 免费asp空间申请 godaddy空间 japanese50m咸熟 ncp 美国十大啦 globalsign web是什么意思 免费网站加速 ipower 建站论坛 阿里云宕机故障 更多