tolerancejavlibrary.com

javlibrary.com  时间:2021-03-19  阅读:()
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions1CanineemotionsasseenthroughhumansocialcognitionMiiamaariaV.
KujalaDepartmentofEquineandSmallAnimalMedicine,UniversityofHelsinki,FinlandDepartmentofNeuroscienceandBiomedicalEngineering,AaltoUniversity,FinlandAbstract:Itisnotpossibletodemonstratethatdogs(Canisfamiliaris)feelemotions,butthesameistrueforallotherspecies,includingourown.
Theissuemustthereforebeapproachedindirectly,usingpremisessimilartothoseusedwithhumans.
Recentmethodologicaladvancesincanineresearchrevealwhatdogsexperienceandwhattheyderivefromtheemotionsperceptibleinothers.
Dogsattendtosocialcues,theyrespondappropriatelytothevalenceofhumananddogfacialexpressionsandvocalizationsofemotion,andtheirlimbicrewardregionsrespondtotheodoroftheircaretakers.
Theybehavedifferentlyaccordingtotheemotionalsituation,showemotionallydrivenexpectations,haveaffectivedisorders,andexhibitsomesubcomponentsofempathy.
Thecaninebrainincludesarelativelylargeprefrontalcortex,andlikeprimates,dogshaveabrainareaspecializedforfaceperception.
Dogshavemanydegreesofemotion,butthefullextentofdogemotionsremainsunknown.
Humansareasociallymindedspecies;wereadilyimputemindandemotiontoothers,eventovegetablesorrocks.
Hencetheexperimentalresultsneedtobeanalyzedcarefully,sotheemotionallivesofdogsareaccuratelyestimated.
Keywords:Canisfamiliaris,domesticdog,emotion,psychology,socialcognition,comparativecognition,neuroscienceMiiamaariaV.
Kujala(Saarela)isaresearcherincomparativecognitionattheUniversityofHelsinki,FacultyofVeterinaryMedicine,DepartmentofEquineandSmallAnimalMedicine,Helsinki,Finland;andAaltoUniversitySchoolofScience,DepartmentofNeuroscienceandBiomedicalEngineering,Espoo,Finland.
Asocialcognitiveneuroscientist,hercurrentresearchinterestsaresocialcognitionandemotionalexperienceindogs,non-invasivebrainresearchacrossspecies,andhumanperceptionandinterpretationofanimals.
http://orcid.
org/0000-0002-1377-2187CallforCommentary:AnimalSentiencepublishesOpenPeerCommentaryonallacceptedtargetarticles.
Targetarticlesarepeer-reviewed.
Commentariesareeditoriallyreviewed.
Therearesubmittedcommentariesaswellasinvitedcommentaries.
Commentariesappearassoonastheyhavebeenreviewed,revisedandaccepted.
Targetarticleauthorsmayrespondtotheircommentariesindividuallyorinajointresponsetomultiplecommentaries.
Instructions:http://animalstudiesrepository.
org/animsent/guidelines.
htmlCallforCommentary:AnimalSentiencepublishesOpenPeerCommentaryonallacceptedtargetarticles.
Targetarticlesarepeer-reviewed.
Commentariesareeditoriallyreviewed.
Therearesubmittedcommentariesaswellasinvitedcommentaries.
Commentariesappearassoonastheyhavebeenreviewed,revisedandaccepted.
Targetarticleauthorsmayrespondtotheircommentariesindividuallyorinajointresponsetomultiplecommentaries.
Instructions:http://animalstudiesrepository.
org/animsent/guidelines.
htmlAnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions21.
IntroductionDogsareourage-olddomesticatedcompanions.
Darwin(1872)considereddogsasacomparativeexampleinhisworkonemotionalexpressionacrossspecies.
Sharingthelivingenvironmentwithus,dogshavedevelopedremarkablesocialskillsininter-speciescommunication(fortheoriginalarticles,seeHareetal.
1998,Sopronietal.
2001,Calletal.
2003,Miklósietal.
2003,Kaminskietal.
2009).
Thus,itisnosurprisethatwehaveinherentinterestinunderstandingdogexperience,behavior,andcognition.
However,provingthatsomeoneexperiencessomethingisanimpossibilitybecauseexperiencesaresubjective(Nagel1974).
Wecanneverknowthatanotherpersonexperiencesthesamethingasus.
Therearealwayssubtledifferencesintheunderlyingpsychologyandphysiology,althoughthelarger-scaleresponsesmaybesimilarwithinaspecies.
How,then,canweknowanythingabouttheinnerlivesofotherspeciessuchasourcompaniondogsInhumanemotionresearch,"emotion"and"feeling"areoftenseparatedbecauseemotioncanbetargetedforobjectiveexperimentalstudythroughbehavioralandneurophysiologicobservations,whereasfeelingissubjective:Whattheemotionfeelslikeandhowitisinterpretedbythesubjectcanonlybeinferredindirectly.
Wecandetectthebehavioralandphysiologicalcorrelatesofbothmyhappinessandyourhappiness,buttheycanbefeltandinterpretedverydifferentlybyeachofus.
Inthiswork,thetopicofcanineemotionsisapproachedusingtheframeworkofAndersonandAdolphs(2014).
Theyarguethatthecapacityforemotioncanexistacrossphylogeny,butemotionsmayconsistofadifferentsetofparallelbehavioral,somatic,physiological,andcognitiveresponsesindifferentspecies.
Webeginbyconsideringtheeffectofthehumanviewpoint.
Canineemotionsareexaminedbothbehaviorallyandbiologically,withabriefreviewoftheneuralbasisforprimaryemotionsandtherespectivestructuresinthedogbrain.
Theneuralbasisforsecondaryemotionsisalsoreviewed,followedbyadiscussionofthecurrentresearchondogs.
Long-termmoodsandcomparativeaspectsarealsoconsidered.
Theultimatepurposeofthistargetarticleistostimulatediscussionaboutthenatureandextentofdogemotionsandtheneedforthisnewfieldofresearch,aswellastoprovidegroundworkfortheapproachfromvariousscientificdisciplines.
Oneofthedifficultiesinconsideringemotionalstatesindogsistheinconsistencyinterminologyacrossstudies.
Avoidinganthropomorphictermshasleftmanycanineaffectivephenomenawithoutastandardizedterminology.
Differentresearchershaveuseddifferenttermsforthesamephenomenon,orsimilartermsforseparatephenomena.
Thisreviewattemptstointegrateresultsacrossstudiesanddisciplines.
2.
HumansocialcognitionaffectsperceptionofdogemotionalstatesTheexistenceofemotionsindogsandtheperceptionofdogemotionsbythehumancaretakersareseparateissues.
Everydaylifepresentsmanypossibilitiesforhumanstomisinterpretthemindbehindadog'sbehavior.
Forexample,guardiansmaymisinterpretthedog'saffectivestateinseparation-relatedanxiety(seeMendletal.
2010a).
Accordingtothethree-factortheoryofanthropomorphism(Epleyetal.
2007),behavioralinterpretationsthatareoftenvalidwithotherhumansarealsoeasilyattributedtonon-humananimalssuchasAnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions3dogs.
Thus,human,biologicallytunedsocialperceptionisthestartingpoint,asitfiltersourunderstandingofdogemotions.
Peoplebelievethatanimalssuchasdogsexperienceemotions(Morrisetal.
2008,Morrisetal.
2012,Walkeretal.
2014).
Humansarealsoquiteconsistentinclassifyingdogs'emotionalbehaviorindifferentcontexts(Pongráczetal.
2005,Tami&Gallagher2009,Walkeretal.
2010,Bucklandetal.
2014,Faragóetal.
2014,Lakestanietal.
2014).
Humansfriendlydogbehaviormosteasilyrecognize,whereasaggressionandfeararemoredifficulttoidentify(Tami&Gallagher2009,Wanetal.
2012,Mirkóetal.
2013,Lakestanietal.
2014)—especiallybychildren(Meintsetal.
2010,Lakestanietal.
2014).
Priorexperienceofdogbehaviourandtraining,ratherthanmereguardianship,enhancestheinterpretationofcaninebehaviourfromthewhole-bodycues(Kujalaetal.
2012,Wanetal.
2012).
Althoughemotionsarevisiblethroughoutdogbodilycues,humanattentionisgenerallydrawntothefacesofbothhumans(Johnsonetal.
1991)anddogs(Quinnetal.
2009).
Humanscanclassifyadog'semotionalvalence(positivity-negativity)fromthefaceirrespectiveofpriorexperiencewithdogs(Bloom&Friedman2013,Schirmeretal.
2013).
Theycandistinguishhappiness(88%ofthetime)andanger/aggressiveness(70%)fromadog'sface,butdiscriminationofotherdiscreteexpressionsislessreliable(fear:45%,sadness:37%,surprise:20%,anddisgust:13%;Bloom&Friedman2013).
Perceptionofothersisaffectedbymanyfactorsinthehumanmind.
Thehumansocialmindisequippedwithapresuppositionofintentionality(forreviews,Blytheetal.
1999,Scholl&Tremoulet2000,Urquiza-Haas&Kotrschal2015),fromwhichanthropomorphismcanarise.
Attributingintentionalityorotherhumancharacteristicstonon-livingthingsisstrengthenedbypersonalconnection(Kiesleretal.
2006),andmentalattributionisfoundinpeople'sdescriptionsofrocks(Kiesler&Kiesler2005),computers(e.
g.
,Nassetal.
1994),animations(Chaminadeetal.
2007),robots(Gazzolaetal.
2007,Imamuraetal.
2015,Martinietal.
2016),orevenvegetables(Vaesetal.
2016).
Humansalsoprojecttheirviewsofthemselvesontodogsmuchastheydowithconspecifics,andtheirperceptionsofdogsaresimilarlyaffectedbystereotypes(Kwanetal.
2008).
Thus,humanseasilyattributementalandemotionalstatestocompaniondogs,andhumaninterpretationofcanineemotionsisfilteredbyhumanpsychologicalcharacteristics(Kujalaetal.
2017).
Humanscanalsodenyhumanityinotherhumans(forreviews,Leyensetal.
2000,Haslam2006).
Theyconsistentlyattributemorecomplexemotionstotheirin-groupthanout-groupmembers(seeLeyensetal.
2000).
Inahumanbrainimagingstudy,theobservationofimagesofextremeout-groupmembers(suchasdrugaddictsorthehomeless)failedtoproducethemedialprefrontalcortexactivationconnectedwithsocialcognition(Harris&Fiske2006).
Thus,thehumanmindisaffectedbyvarioussocialfactors,withthejudgmentssometimesrepresentingmorethejudge'sownideologythanthereality.
Likewise,whenhumansattempttodeciphercanineemotions,dogguardianscanunderestimatetheirdogs'aggressiveness(Mirkóetal.
2013).
Attributingmindstoothersisinnateinhumans,andthehumanbrainappearsremarkablyflexibleregardingthesourceoftheothermind.
Humanempathygeneralizestootherspecies(Ascione1992,Paul2000,Taylor&Signal2005,Norringetal.
2014,WestburyInghametal.
2015,Kujalaetal.
2017)andaffectsourinterpretationofdogbehavior(Meyer&Forkman2014,Meyeretal.
2014).
Empathy(WestburyInghametal.
2015)andtheattributionofmentalstates(Harrison&Hall2010)tonon-humananimalsvarieswiththeirAnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions4phylogeneticrelatednesstohumans.
Mentalattributiontobothhumanandnon-humanspeciesisconnectedtothetemporoparietaljunctionassociatedwithhumantheory-of-mindabilities(Cullenetal.
2014).
Humanbrainresponsestodogscanalsobestrikinglysimilartoresponsestohumanconspecifics,whetherobservingdogs'facialexpressions(Spuntetal.
2016),pain(Franklinetal.
2013),orsocialinteraction(Kujalaetal.
2012).
Humanbrainsseekothermindsandemotions,anddogemotionalbehaviourisfilteredthroughthesamemachinery.
3.
Neuralsupportforthebasic(primary)canineemotionsItisgenerallyagreedthatbasicemotionalstatessuchasanger,happiness,andfearareevolutionarilyadaptive(Ekman1992,Izard1992,Panksepp1998,Plutchik2001),andtheyhaveuniversalfacialexpressionpatternsinhumans(Ekman&Friesen1971).
Basicemotionalstatesareassociatedwithneuralstructureswithinthelimbicsystemanditsconnectionstotheneocortexinmammals(Damasio1994;LeDoux1996;Rolls1999).
Specificbasicemotionsareassociatedwithspecificchemicalneurotransmitterbalanceinthebrain(Panksepp1998).
Figure1.
Keyareasofthedogbrain.
(A)Somekeysubcorticalareasshownontheaxialmagneticresonanceimagingslices(dog'snosepointingupwards;top=anterior,bottom=posterior,left=lefthemisphere,andright=righthemisphere)and(B)thecorticalsurfaceofthedogbrain,withthesulciopenedandthenomenclatureoverlaidbelow.
Thecorticalsurfaceisshownlaterallyfromthelefthemisphere(dog'snosepointingleft;left=anterior,right=posterior,top=dorsal,andbottom=ventral);theimagehasbeenmagnifiedwithstandarddigitalimageprocessingtoshowthegyriandsulci.
Modified,underthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttributionLicense,fromfigureshttp://dx.
doi.
org/10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0052140.
g002andhttp://dx.
doi.
org/10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0052140.
g003(Dattaetal.
2012).
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions5Thedomesticdog,asamemberofmammalianorderCarnivoraandtheCaninaefamily,hasabrainthatincludesallthemajorstructuresandconnectionssupportingbasicemotionalfunctions(Jensen2007,deLahunta&Glass2009,Evans&deLahunta2013).
Dogbrainsinclude,bilaterally,thelimbicsystemwiththenucleusaccumbens;theamygdalawithitssensoryandcorticalconnections;thecingulatecortex;andthesensory-motorcorticesandtheinsula,deepwithinthepseudosylvianfissure(inhumans,theSylvianfissureoccupiesthetopologicallyequivalentposition).
Dogsalsohavearelativelylargeprefrontalcortexthatisnotdirectlyassociatedwithmotorfunctions(seeFigure1,Palazzi2011,Dattaetal.
2012,Evans&deLahunta2013).
Thecorrespondingstructuresinhumanshavebeenstudiedextensivelyinrecentdecadesregardingtheirrolesinsocialandemotionalfunction(forreviews,seee.
g.
,Bushetal.
2000,Damasioetal.
2000,Adolphs2002,Leppnen&Nelson2009,Etkinetal.
2011,Schilbachetal.
2013).
Fordogs,theresearchismorescattered.
Somedogbrainfunctionisalsoinferredfromneurologicalexperimentswithcats(Feliscatus)andthehomologuesbetweenthebrainanatomiesofthetwospecies.
Manyfunctionsofthesubcorticalnuclei(e.
g.
,theseptalareaorhypothalamus)andthefinerneurophysiologicdetails,aswellasvisualcorticalorganization,areinferredfromcatstudies(King1987,deLahunta&Glass2009,Sjaastadetal.
2010).
Utilizationofthemethodologyforstudyinghumanbrainfunctionhasfacilitatedthestudyofdogbrains.
Arecentstudyshowedthatdogs'nucleusaccumbensisactivatedbytheodoroffamiliarhumans,highlightingthepossibilitiesofmethodologicaladvancesinexaminingdogemotions(Bernsetal.
2015).
Dogandhumanbrainsalsohaveimportantdifferences.
Theassociationareas(brainareasnotdirectlyresponsibleforsensomotorfunctions)coverabout20%ofthedogneocortexbut85%ofthehumanneocortex(Evans&deLahunta2013).
Therhinencephalon,devotedtoprocessingolfactorysignals,coversarelativelylargeareaindogbrains(Evans&deLahunta2013).
Theexistenceoflimbicandcorticalstructuresindogsisconsistentwithhavingthebasicemotions,althoughdogs'qualia–whatitfeelsliketobeadog(Nagel1974)—nodoubtdifferfromourown.
4.
Emotionalreactivityandaffective-behavioraldisordersindogsResearchondogemotionshastraditionallyconcentratedontheproblemsdogbehaviorcausesforthehumanguardians,whichiswhyweknowmoreaboutdogfearandaggression.
Dogemotionalityhasbeenstudiedtopredictgeneralemotionalreactivity(Goddard&Beilharz1986,Sforzinietal.
2009),aggression(Netto&Planta1997,vandenBergetal.
2003,vanderBorgetal.
2010),behavioraldisorders(vanderBorgetal.
1991),anddifferencesamongdogbreeds(Scott&Fuller1965)frompuppiestoadulthood.
Dogaggressivenessistestedbypresentingprovocativestimuli,suchasanunfamiliarbarkingdog(Netto&Planta1997,vandenBergetal.
2003)orstaringthedogintheeyes(Sforzinietal.
2009).
Guardianquestionnairesarealsoused(Netto&Planta1997,Hsu&Serpell2003,Duffyetal.
2008).
Similarly,testingadog'sfearfulnesscanincludepresentingasuddenloudnoise,anovelobject,afallingbag,oragunshot(Melzack1952,Beerdaetal.
1998,Kingetal.
2003,Hydbring-Sandbergetal.
2004,Morrowetal.
2015).
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions6Bothfearfulandaggressivebehaviorsindogsareassociatedwithsomephysiologicalorautonomicresponses.
Stimulielicitingfearfulbehaviorindogsincreasetheircortisol(Beerdaetal.
1998,Kingetal.
2003,Hydbring-Sandbergetal.
2004,Dreschel&Granger2005,Morrowetal.
2015)orprogesteronelevels(Hydbring-Sandbergetal.
2004),heartrates(Kingetal.
2003,Hydbring-Sandbergetal.
2004,Ogataetal.
2006),andbodytemperatures(Ogataetal.
2006).
Aggressivebehaviorsareassociatedwithreducedserotonergicfunction(Reisneretal.
1996).
Themostcommonaffective-behavioralclinicallytreateddisordersindogsarerelatedtofearfuloraggressivebehaviorandmaybeinducedbyseparationanxiety,noisesensitivity(forreview,Sherman&Mills2008),anddominance/competitiveaggression(seee.
g.
,Beaver1983,Wright&Nesselrote1987,Cameron1997,Reisner1997,Haug2008).
Treatmentsfortheseconditionsusuallyincludebehaviormodification,oftencombinedwithneuropharmacologicalmedicationasinhumanpsychiatricdisorders(Overall2000).
Takentogether,aggressionandfeararethemoststudiedemotionsindogs,butresearchonotheremotionalstatesisscarce.
Anexceptionamongthepositiveemotionsisdogplaybehavior,whichiswell-documented(Bekoff1974a,Bekoff1974b,Bekoff1995,Rooneyetal.
2000,Horvathetal.
2008,Wardetal.
2008,Horowitz2009a,Palagietal.
2015).
5.
ProductionandperceptionoffacialexpressionsFacesandfacialexpressionsconveydelicateandmeaningfulinformationaboutemotionalstatestoconspecificsinhumans(forreviews,Adolphs2002,Calder&Young2005,Hari&Kujala2009,Leppnen&Nelson2009)aswellasinmanynon-humanspecies(forreviews,Tateetal.
2006;Leopold&Rhodes2010).
FacialexpressionsofemotionindogswerediscussedbyDarwin(1872);theycharacterizedingreatdetailsincethe1960s,notingsimilaritiesintheemotionalexpressionsforaggressionandhappinessbetweendogs,othercarnivores,andprimates(Bolwig1964,Fox1970).
Aprecisecodingofhumanfacialexpressionsbasedonthemovementoffacialmuscles—afacialactioncodingsystem(FACS)—wasdevelopedinthe1970s(Ekman&Friesen1978).
Thesystemhassincebeenappliedtomanyotherprimatespecies(Vicketal.
2007,Parretal.
2010,Walleretal.
2012,Caeiroetal.
2013),horses(Equuscaballus,Wathanetal.
2015),cats(http://www.
catfacs.
com/),anddogs(Walleretal.
2013).
Deviatingfromthehuman-FACS,thenon-human-FACSoftenincludesthemovementofears.
Behavioralandbrainresponsesduringtheperceptionoffacialexpressionshavebeenstudiedinnon-humanprimatesandsheepfordecades(seeTateetal.
2006).
Asasecondnon-primatespeciesaftersheep(Kendrick&Baldwin1987),dogshavebeenshowntopossessadistinguishableface-processingregioninthetemporalcortex,separatingbrainresponsestofacesfromtheresponsestoobjects(Figure2)(Dilksetal.
2015,Cuayaetal.
2016).
Theresponseprofilesareroughlycomparablewiththoseofthehumanfusiformfacearea(Kanwisheretal.
1997),althoughthecorticalregionseemstobemorevariableindogs.
Inhumans,faceprocessingcontinuesfromthefusiformtotheinferotemporalcortexandthesuperiortemporalsulcus,withtheextractionofidentity-andemotion-specificinformation(forreview,Haxbyetal.
2000).
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions7Figure2.
Strongerbrainresponsesfromdogsforfacesversusobjects.
Imagesshowthefocusofbrainactivationduringnon-invasivefunctionalmagneticresonanceimaginginsevendogsforfacesversusobjectswithcontrast,overlaidonadigitallyproducedglassbraintorevealfocilocatedwithinthesulci.
(A)Lateralviewfromlefthemisphere,(B)rostralviewfromfront,and(C)lateralviewfromrighthemisphere.
A=anterior,P=posterior,S=superior(ordorsal),I=inferior(andventral),L=left,andR=right.
Modified,underthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttributionLicense,fromfigurehttp://dx.
doi.
org/10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0149431.
g005(Cuayaetal.
2016).
Whiledirectinformationondogs'brainprocessingofemotionalexpressionsismissing,agrowingbodyofbehavioralandeye-trackingresearchsupportstheabilityofdogstodistinguishnegativeandpositivefacialexpressionsinbothhumansanddogs,andtorespondappropriatelyaccordingtothevalenceoffaces(Nagasawaetal.
2011,Raccaetal.
2012,Mülleretal.
2015,Barberetal.
2016,Somppietal.
2016)(Figure3).
Figure3.
Gazefixations(circles)andscanningpaths(linesbetweenthecircles)oftwodogs(showninlightanddarkgreen)forfacialexpressionsofdogsandhumans.
Whitecirclesrepresentthetargetsofthefirstfixations;dogstendtogazefirstintotheeyesofbothhumansanddogs.
Figurefromhttp://dx.
doi.
org/10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0149431.
g005(Somppietal.
2016),reprintedunderthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttributionLicense.
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions8Dogsassociateemotionalvocalizationsofbothhumansanddogswiththecorrespondingfacialexpressions,showingmultisensoryprocessingofemotionalexpressions(Albuquerqueetal.
2016).
Likehumaninfants,dogsusehumanemotionalexpressionsforsocialreferencing,asasourceofapproach/avoidinformationfornovelobjects(Merolaetal.
2012b,Merolaetal.
2012a,Buttelmann&Tomasello2013,Merolaetal.
2014,Turcsánetal.
2015).
Furthermore,theyappeartogeneralizethevalenceinformationoffacialexpressionsacrosshumanindividuals(Mülleretal.
2015,Somppietal.
2016)ratherthanrespondingonlytoguardians'expressions—incontrasttocats,whorespondmainlytothevalenceoftheirguardians'facialexpressions(Galvan&Vonk2016).
Humancross-culturalstudiescouldprovidesomeusefulcluesforstudyingemotionsindogs.
Thebasicemotionsareremarkablysimilararoundtheworld(Ekman&Friesen1971).
Facialexpressionsandtheirrecognition,situationsprovokingemotions,andtheorganizationofemotionsonthevalence-arousaldimensionsareconsistentacrosscultures(Shaveretal.
1992).
However,theperceptionofemotionalintensitydiffersacrosscultures(Ekmanetal.
1987),andthedecodingandencodingofemotionintheculturalin-groupappearsmoreaccurate(formeta-analysis,Elfenbein&Ambady2002).
Similarlytotheculturaldifferencesinhumans,differencesinbreedsorenvironmentmayaffecttheexpressionorperceptionofemotionindogs(forreview,Mehrkam&Wynne2014).
Itappearsunquestionablethatdogscanbothproduceandprocessemotionsthroughfacialexpression,butthequestionistowhatextentOneremainingquestionconcernswhatpartofrecognizinghumanfacialexpressionsbydogsisinnateandwhatislearnedthroughassociationandexperience.
Also,theresearchaddressedmainlythepositive-negativevalenceinformationoffacesratherthanthemorediverse,discreteexpressionsofemotion(e.
g.
,happiness,sadness,surprise,fear,disgust,anger:Ekman&Friesen1971),soinformationondogs'abilitytodiscriminateorrespondtodiscreteexpressionsofemotionislacking.
Arethereuniversalfacialexpressionsofemotionindogsasthereareinhumans(Ekman&Friesen1971)—andifso,whatarethey6.
Fundamentalbasisforsecondary(social)emotionsSecondaryemotions—generatedthroughtheinterpretationofsocialsituationsandrequiringsomesenseofanother'smind—arelesslikelythanbasic(primary)emotionstobeattributedtodogsbypeople,but22to94%ofpeoplebelievethatdogsdohavesecondaryemotionssuchasshameorguilt(Morrisetal.
2008,Morrisetal.
2012).
Asadulthumans,weeffortlesslyattributesecondaryemotionstootherpeople.
Withoutknowledgeofthedifferencesamongmindsacrossspecies,wecanjustaseasilyattributetheemotionstonon-humans,includingdogs.
However,dogsmaybeincapableofexperiencingthemorecomplexsocialemotions,ortheirexperiencesmaybequalitativelyverydifferentfromours.
Thereasonthedogishuman'sbestfriendmaybetheapparentlymissingcaninecapacityforsecondaryemotionssuchascontemptorSchadenfreude(thejoyinothers'misfortune).
Thesecondaryemotionsseemtorequiresomesenseoftheself(Leary2003).
Havingself-awarenesscomplicatestheemotionalexperienceinmanyways—allowingimaginedexperienceswithnobasisinreality.
Leary(2003)clarifiestheeffectofselfonemotionalexperiencethroughfivepoints:"Specifically,havingaselfpermitspeopleto(1)evokeemotionsinthemselvesbyimagingself-relevantevents,(2)reactemotionallytoabstractandAnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions9symbolicimagesofthemselvesintheirownminds,(3)consciouslycontemplatethecauseoftheiremotions,(4)experienceemotionsbythinkingabouthowtheyareperceivedbyotherpeople,and(5)deliberatelyregulatetheiremotionalexperience"(p.
775).
Ashealsopointsout,animalsdonotneedaconceptofselfinordertohaveabasicemotion.
Humansoftenattempttosuppressself-referentialemotionalthoughtinvariousways(e.
g.
,bydrinkingalcohol)sincesuchthoughtscancauseincreasingdistress.
Inchildren,self-awarenessappearstoariseroughlyconcurrentlywiththeabilitytotakeanother'sperspective(Lempersetal.
1977);earlystudiessuggestsimilarco-occurrencesinotherspecies(Gallup&Suarez1986).
Todate,thelevelofdogs'self-awarenessisnotknown—forexample,theyhavenotpassedGallup's(1986)mirrorself-recognitiontest,buttheydospendlesstimeinspectingtheirownurinemarkingsthanthoseofothers(Bekoff2001).
Thebrainregionsresponsibleforsecondaryemotionsincludeanetworkcomprisingthemedialorbitofrontalcortex,thetemporalpole,andthesuperiortemporalsulcusinhumans(Molletal.
2002,Burnett&Blakemore2009).
Inprinciple,homologuesoftheseregionsmayalsobepresentinthebrainsofdogs,inthetemporalandfrontalassociationareas.
Homologuesincorticesaredifficulttoverifybecausethebrainfunctionsofthesecorticalregionscannotbelocalizedanatomically,thusfunctionalbrainimagingisneeded.
Nevertheless,thecorticalassociationareas,associatedwithsecondaryemotionsinhumans,arelargerinhumansthanindogs(20%ofthecortexindogsand85%inhumans;Evans&deLahunta2013).
Thebrainareasrespondingtosecondaryemotionsarealsostronglyconnectedtoareasofthelimbicsystem,andtheconnectionsalterthelevelofcognitiveevaluationoftheemotionalstates(Berridge2003).
Theconnectionsbetweenthecortexandthelimbicsystemaresodifferentinmagnitudebetweenhumansandothermammalsthatcorticallesionshavingminimaleffectsonothermammalsmaycausedrasticchangesinhumanfunction(Berridge2003).
Forexample,acatwithoutacortexmaystillmoveandbehavelikeacat,whereasahumanwithoutacortex,ifalive,liesinahospitalbedcompletelyunresponsive.
Thus,itispossiblethatthere-representationofemotionsthathumanencephalizationproduceswiththeinterconnectionstothelimbicsystemmaybethesourceofsecondaryemotions.
Inotherwords,ashumanswehavethepotentialtobeangry,realizethatweareangry,ponderthecausesofouranger,noticethattheangermomentarilyaffectsourabilitytoworkorcooperate,trytosuppressouranger,thinkabouthowourangerappearstoourcompanionsandhowitaffectsourrelationships,andtrytomodifythesourceoftheanger.
Ifthecerebral-limbicinterconnectionsarethesourceofthisemotionalre-representationalabilityinhumans,theoverallcapacityofdogsforsecondaryemotions,withlessencephalizationthanhumans,maybedramaticallydifferentfromours.
7.
Dodogsdisplayguilt—ormerelyappeasementGuiltisanexampleofasecondaryemotionoftenattributedtodogs,butaccordingtocurrentresearch,itfitsthedogmindpoorly.
Horowitz(2009b)firstrecordedcaninebehaviorandgesturalcuesinasituationwheredogscoulddisobeytheguardian'scommandandeataforbiddentreat.
Bymanipulatingtheguardian'sbeliefaboutwhathappenedinthesituation,sheshowedthatdogs'gestureswerenotdifferentwhetherornottheyobeyed.
Instead,thegesturescommonlyassociatedwithdog"guilt"—forexample,avoidingeyecontact,waggingAnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions10thetaillowandquickly,holdingone'searsorheaddown—wereevidentwhentheguardiansscoldedtheirdogs,regardlessofthedog'sactualbehaviorintheexperiment.
Thisstronglysuggestedthatthedogsrespondedtotheguardian'sbehaviorwithsubmissivegesturesinterpretedbydogguardiansas"guilt,"ratherthandisplayingremorseforamisdeedwiththe"guilty"gestures(Horowitz2009b).
Afteradoghaslearnedtheassociationbetweenacertainunwantedbehavior(e.
g.
,stealingfood)andtheguardian'spunishmentlater,theymaydisplaythesubmissivebehaviorinasimilarsituationevenbeforetheguardian'sscolding(Horowitz2009b).
Thisdoesnotrequireremorseoran"understanding"ofviolatinganorm,butasimplelearnedassociationbetweentwosuccessivesituations.
Whendogsdisplay"guilt"behavior,guardiansarelikelytoscoldtheirdogless,whichsuggeststhat"guilt"behaviormayfunctionaslearnedappeasement(Hechtetal.
2012).
Inarecentwork,petdogs'heartratesweremeasuredduringthe"forbiddentreat"experiment,anddogswhotooktheforbiddentreathadahigherheartratethandogswhodidnot(Torres-Pereira&Broom2014).
Thissuggestsalearnedassociationbetweeneatingthetreatandapossibleconsequentscolding.
Toensurethattheriseinheartratewasnotmerelyafunctionofsympatheticnervoussystemactivationintheactivecondition(eatingaforbiddentreat),asimilarresultshouldbeobtainedwithdogseatingatreatthattheywereallowed.
Alternatively,asbothpositiveandnegativestresscanincreasesympatheticnervoussystemactivation,thetreat-stealingdogsmayjustbemoreexcitedbythetreat.
Nevertheless,evenrepresentationofthecausalityoftheactionplusananticipatoryresponsetotheconsequencedoesnotrequireasenseofguilt.
8.
FairnessorunequaltreatmentofconspecificsAnotherexampleofdogaffectiverepresentationsclosertothesecondaryemotionsisinequityaversion(Figure4).
Instudiesofprimatesocialcooperation,unequaltreatmentofindividualsisrelatedtonegativeresponses(forreview,deWaal&Suchak2010).
Inhumans,thefeelingofinequalityischaracterizedbyanegativeresponsetotheviolationoffairness(forreviews,seeFehr&Rockenbach2004,Fehr&Camerer2007).
Adegreeofinequityaversionwasreportedincapuchinmonkeys(Cebusapella,Brosnan&DeWaal2003)andchimpanzees(Pantroglodytes,Brosnanetal.
2005),andafewstudieshaveinvestigatedthephenomenonindogs(Rangeetal.
2009,Horowitz2012,Rangeetal.
2012,Brucksetal.
2016).
Inasituationwhereaconspecificpartnerwasrewardedforataskandcaninesubjectswerenot,theyrefusedtoperformthetaskorhesitatedlonger(Rangeetal.
2009,Brucksetal.
2016).
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions11Figure4.
Testingdoginequityaversion;bothdogsareaskedtogivethepawinturn.
Figurefromhttp://dx.
doi.
org/10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0153799.
g001(Brucksetal.
2016),reprintedunderthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttributionLicense.
Thephenomenondoesnotqualifyasasimpleextinctionofalearnedbehaviorbecausethecaninesubjectsrefusedtoobeyearlierafterwitnessingapartnerreceivearewardforobeying,comparedtobeingalone(Rangeetal.
2009,Rangeetal.
2012,Brucksetal.
2016).
Theyalsotendedtorefuseearlierintheunequalsituationthaninthesituationwhereneitherdogreceivedrewards(Rangeetal.
2009,Rangeetal.
2012).
Dogsalsosharedtheirfoodandinteractedlesswithpartnersafterunequalsituations,showingthatthenegativeexperienceofunequaltreatment,ornotbeingrewardedforone'sefforts,diminishessubsequentcooperationandtoleranceofcompany(Brucksetal.
2016).
Humansandsomeotherprimatescanresistunequaltreatment(i.
e.
,refusetocooperate)eitherwhentheygainlessthanthepartnerorwhentheygainmore(Brosnanetal.
2010,Blake&McAuliffe2011).
Incontrast,dogsdonotresisttheinequitywhentheyaremorerewardedthantheircompanions(Horowitz2012).
Takentogether,dogsaresensitivetoconspecificcompanyintheinequityaversiontest.
Theyrefuseearliertoperformthetaskinsituationswhentheyreceivefewerrewardsthantheirpartner,comparedtowhentheyarealone,butunlikesomeprimates,theydonotresistgainingmorethanthepartner.
Althoughdogsbehavedifferentlytowardthecompanionandexperimenterafterunequalandequalconditions(Brucksetal.
2016),thiscouldalsoreflectthenegativeoverallmoodcreatedbynotbeingrewarded.
Thedatatodatesuggestthatdogshavethecapacityforinequityaversion,butfutureworkisneededwithmoreconditionssuchasvaryingfoodquantitiesandthedirectionoftheinequality,learningthroughasocialmodel,expectationviolation,negativesituationsaffectingsubsequentbehavior,andindividualfactorssuchaspersonalityorbreed.
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions129.
ResourcecompetitionasaprecursorofjealousyAnothercaseofpossiblesecondaryemotionsindogsisjealousy.
Dogguardiansreportbehaviorsrelatedtojealousyasoftenasbehaviorsrelatedtothebasicemotionofanger(Morrisetal.
2008).
However,thisalsoillustratesanthropomorphicmisunderstandings:Couplesmayreportthat"Ontherareoccasionthatwehaveacuddlehe'llstartbarkingandwhining.
"Buthuggingisnotindogs'naturalbehaviorrepertoire,sopeoplecuddlingcanappeartodogsasathreatbetweenpackmembers,towhichtheyreactbywhiningortryingtoseparatethe"fightingpackmembers.
"Inhumans,jealousyusuallyconcernsromanticrelationshipsandextendstoimaginarysituationsofarivalthreateningasignificantrelationship(Leary2003).
Aprecursorofjealousyindogsmayexistinasituationofdefendingapreviouslygainedresourcesuchasahumancompanion.
Inarecentbehavioralstudywheredogguardiansignoredtheirdogsandattendedtorealistictoydogsorotherobjects,thedogsexhibitedsignificantlymorebehaviorssuchasgoingbetweentheguardianandthetargetoftheirattention,orpushing/touchingtheguardianorthetarget,whenthetargetwasarealistictoydogratherthananobject(Harris&Prouvost2014).
Similarly,humaninfantsshowedmorenegativeresponseswhenamother'sattentionwasdirectedtowardsalife-likedollthananobjectlikeabook(Hartetal.
1998,Hart&Carrington2002).
Althoughthecurrentdataareconsistentwiththepossibilityofsituation-basedresourcedefensebeingaprecursorofjealousy,theevidenceforenvyorjealousyindogsisinconclusiveandmorerigorousresearchisrequired.
Unfortunately,thebehaviorsassociatedwithdogjealousycanalsoappearindogsasreplacementbehaviors,whenthedogisconfusedastohowtoreact.
10.
DivisionsofempathyTheneuroscientificstudyofhumanempathyexplodedinthebeginningofthe2000s(seeSingeretal.
2004,Jacksonetal.
2005,Gazzolaetal.
2006,Saarelaetal.
2007),revealingthattheemotionalaspectofempathyisprocessedinthelimbicsystem,insula,andanteriorcingulatecortex.
Similarpatternsmaybealsopossibleinthecaninebrain.
Althoughsimpleformsofnon-humanempathyhadbeenstudiedinpreviousdecades(Church1959,Rice&Gainer1962,Massermanetal.
1964,Watanabe&Ono1986),interestinanimalandhumanempathygrewwiththestudyofnon-humanprimates(forreviews,Preston&deWaal2002,deWaal&Ferrari2010),andalsoextendedtonon-primates,includingrats(Rattusnorvegicus,Ben-AmiBartaletal.
2011).
Inhumans,empathyhasthreecomponents:emotionalempathy,cognitiveempathy,andtheseparationoftheselffromtheother(seeDecety&Jackson2004).
Emotionalempathycanbefurtherdividedintoemotionalcontagion/self-distressandempathicconcern:emotionalcontagionoriginatesfromautomaticallytriggeredresponsestoothers'emotions,andempathicconcernincludesexpressingaworryaboutothers'wellbeing(Davis1980).
Emotionalcontagionisimportantforcompassion,butathighlevelsitmayleadtoanxietyandpassivityoraggressionandantisocialityratherthanhelpingbehavior(Roberts&Strayer1996).
Cognitiveempathyinvolvesatheory-of-mind-likemeta-representationofanother'semotionalstate.
Tohighlightthedifferencebetweenemotionalandcognitiveempathy,theAnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions13lattercanbefullypreservedinhumansdiagnosedwithpsychopathictendencies,whereasthispopulationshowsmuchlessemotionalcontagionandempathicconcern(Blair2005),possiblyduetoalteredlimbicfunction(Birbaumeretal.
2005).
11.
FromemotionalcontagiontoprosocialbehaviorindogsAlthoughlongrecognizedbysome(seee.
g.
,Bekoff2007),thecapabilityofdogstoempathizehasbeenreceivingmorescientificattentionrecently.
Anecdotalreportsofdogsapparentlyconsolingconspecificsorhumansareabundant,butthetopichasbeenthoroughlyexaminedonlyinafewexperiments.
Mostresearchondogs'empathy-relatedbehaviortowardconspecificsconcernsbehaviorthatresemblesconsolation,thatis,reconciliationorpost-conflictaffiliation.
Incooperativespecies,aggressiontowardconspecificsmaybecostlyforthewholegroup.
Animportantmechanismformanagingtheeffectsofaggressionispost-conflictaffiliativebehaviorbetweentheformeropponents(reconciliation),sometimesthroughmediationbyathirdparty(deWaal&vanRoosmalen1979).
Bothreconciliationbehaviorandthird-partypost-conflictaffiliationarepresentingroup-livingdogsandwolvesinheightenedgreeting;sittingorlyingdowntogether;andsniffing,playing,orlickingthevictimofaggression(Coolsetal.
2007,Palagi&Cordoni2009,Cordoni&Palagi2015).
Emotionalcontagionacrossdogswasrecentlystudiedbyplayingfamiliarandunfamiliarconspecificwhinestodogsandexaminingtheirbehaviorduringtheplaybackandreunionwiththefamiliardogs(Quervel-Chaumetteetal.
2016).
Whenexposedtodogwhines(recordedwhenthedogwasleftalone),thecaninesubjectsweremorealertandexhibitedmorestress-relatedbehaviorscomparedwithexposuretoacousticallymatchedcontrolsounds(Quervel-Chaumetteetal.
2016).
Additionally,exposuretofamiliardogwhinestriggeredmorecomfort-offeringbehaviorstowardthepartnerdogsinreunion,resemblingpost-conflictaffiliativebehaviorobservedinnaturalgroups(Coolsetal.
2007,Palagi&Cordoni2009,Cordoni&Palagi2015).
Post-conflictaffiliationhighlightsthepossibilityofemotionalcontagionorempathicconcernindogs,althoughitsmechanismsareunknown.
Across-speciesaffiliativeinteractionbetweenadogandtheircaretaker(e.
g.
,guardianpettingthedog)cancausehormonalandphysiologicalsynchronization,loweringcortisollevelsandincreasingoxytocinanddopaminelevelsinbothspecies(Odendaal&Meintjes2003,Milleretal.
2009,Nagasawaetal.
2009,Handlinetal.
2011,Nagasawaetal.
2015).
Thisacross-speciesemotionalsynchronizationsuggestsapossiblephysiologicalmechanismfortheemotionalcontagionbothinhumansanddogs.
Inasimilarexamplerelatedtothestressresponse,cortisollevelsinbothhumansanddogsincreasedsignificantlyafterlisteningtoacryinghumaninfantcomparedwithababblinginfantorwhitenoise(Yong&Ruffman2014).
Dogsmayalsoactprosocially,pullingaropethatdeliversapartnerdogarewardevenwhenthepullerdogsthemselvesarenotrewarded,butonlyiftherecipientdogisfamiliar(Quervel-Chaumetteetal.
2015).
Similarhelpingbehaviorfromdogstohumanswaspreviouslyfound(forreview,Bruer2015).
Theseresultsshowthepossibilityofdogs'altruisticbehavior.
Morestudiesontheextentofthiskindofbehavior.
Overall,thestudiesshowemotionalcontagionfromhumansacrossspeciestodogs,aswellasfromdogstotheirconspecifics,althoughtheunderlyingmechanismsofcontagionarecurrentlynotclear.
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions1412.
Yawningcontagiousnessindogs:EmpathyorsocialrelaxationContagiousyawningisnotusedasameasureofempathyinhumanpsychology,sincewell-validatedquestionnaires(e.
g.
,Mehrabian&Epstein1972,Davis1980,Lawrenceetal.
2004,Daddsetal.
2008)canbecombinedwitheitherbehavioralstudies(especiallyinchildren,seee.
g.
,Eisenberg&Miller1987,Eisenberg&Fabes1990,Roberts&Strayer1996,Eisenbergetal.
1999)orbrainimagingstudies(forreviews,Decety&Jackson2004,Bernhardt&Singer2012)toshowacorrespondencebetweenself-reportedreactivityandbehavioralorphysiologicalchanges.
EarlystudiesreporteddiminishedcontagiousnessofyawninginchildrenwithAutismSpectrumDisorders(ASD)(Senjuetal.
2007,Giganti&EspositoZiello2009,Heltetal.
2010),whichpromptedspeculationregardingthepossiblerelationofcontagiousyawningtoempathy.
NewerstudieshavereportedthecapacityforcontagiousyawninginchildrenwithASD(Senjuetal.
2009,Usuietal.
2013)andtheindependenceofyawningcontagiousnessfromempathy(Bartholomew&Cirulli2014).
However,sincetheinauguralstudyshowingyawncontagionacrossspeciesfromhumanstodogs(Joly-Mascheronietal.
2008),arangeofstudieshasexploredthepossibleconnectionofcontagiousyawningandempathyincanines(O'Hara&Reeve2011,Silvaetal.
2012,Madsen&Persson2013,Romeroetal.
2013,Silvaetal.
2013,Buttner&Strasser2014,Romeroetal.
2014).
Yawningisasomewhatproblematicmeasureindogsbecausethecanineyawnservesasatension-releasingstressresponse(seee.
g.
,Beerdaetal.
1998).
Nevertheless,manystudiesfoundahigherfrequencyofcanineyawningafterobservingorhearingahumanyawn(Joly-Mascheronietal.
2008,Silvaetal.
2012,Madsen&Persson2013,Romeroetal.
2013).
Somestudiesalsoreportedstrongeryawncontagiousnessindogsafterperceivingafamiliarratherthannon-familiarpersonyawning(Silvaetal.
2012,Romeroetal.
2013).
Thiseffectofsocialconnectednesswasalsodemonstratedwithinwolves(Romeroetal.
2014).
Inhumans,thetendencytoyawnafterwitnessinganotherperson'syawnisnegativelycorrelatedwithamygdalaactivation(Schürmannetal.
2005).
Thus,higheramygdalaactivation,whichmayoccurnaturallyinunknowncompanyasvigilanceforathreat(e.
g.
,Whalen1998,Hartetal.
2000),actsagainstyawningcontagion.
Ifasimilarconnectionexistedincanids,familiaritywouldincreasecontagiousyawningmerelyasafunctionoftheindividual'slevelofsocialrelaxedness.
Theyawncontagiousnessindogs,however,addstothepossibilityofinterspecificemotionalsynchronizationandcontagioninrelaxation(Odendaal&Meintjes2003,Milleretal.
2009,Nagasawaetal.
2009,Handlinetal.
2011,Nagasawaetal.
2015).
Altogether,thestudiesofempathyshowthatatleastemotionalcontagionispossibleindogs.
Studiesoncaninepost-conflictaffiliativebehaviorandprosocialityalsosuggestsomeempathicconcern.
Arecentstudyoncanineprosocialtendencies(Quervel-Chaumetteetal.
2015)alsoreportedsituation-dependentperspective-taking.
Theextentofempathiccapacityindogsisunknown,however(fordiscussion,seealsoSilva&deSousa2011).
Thereareprobablylimitstothecognitivecomponentofempathyindogs:Theylackmeta-representationalandself-representationalskillsbecausetheirbrainshavelessencephalizationandconnectivitythanhumans.
However,coupledwiththerudimentarytheoryofmindhypothesizedbyHorowitz(2011),empathicrespondingmaynotbeanall-or-nonefunctionbutanabilitythatoccurstovariousdegreesacrosssocialspecies.
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions1513.
Long-termmoods:A"cognitivejudgmentbias"testTheeffectsofrearingenvironmentonemotionalresponseinanimalswerenotedinthe1950sbypsychologistssuchasHebb(1955).
Raisingdogsinarestrictedenvironmentaffectedtheirsubsequentemotionalresponses(Melzack1954).
Fiftyyearslater,theworkwasextendedintothefieldofanimalwelfare,andtheeffectoftheenvironmentonthepositiveornegativeaffectivestatesinanimalswasstudiedusinganemotionaljudgmentbiastest(Hardingetal.
2004).
Theeffectiscalled"cognitivejudgmentbias"or"cognitivebias"intheanimalsciences(Mendletal.
2010a,Rygulaetal.
2015),althoughhumanpsychologyhasamultitudeofdifferentcognitivebiases(seee.
g.
,Tversky&Kahneman1974,Haseltonetal.
2005).
Thisexpectation-related"cognitivebias"isalsobasedonstudiesofhumansinwhichthephenomenoniswidelyknownasaffectivecongruence(e.
g.
,Bower1991,Fazio2010):anxiousordepressedpeopletendtointerpretambiguousstimulinegatively(Eysencketal.
1991,Wright&Bower1992,MacLeod&Byrne1996,Gotlib&Krasnoperova1998).
Non-humananimalsmayalsobebiasedintheirexpectationsafternegativeorpositiveexperiences(Mendletal.
2010b).
Thebasictestissimple:animalsarefirsttrainedthatonestimulus(e.
g.
,ablackcard)signalsapositiveevent(reward,e.
g.
,food),andanother(awhitecard)signalsanegativeevent(apunishment).
Aftersuchtraining,theyarepresentedwithambiguous,intermediatesignals(e.
g.
,agreycard),andtheirreactions(e.
g.
,timeofapproachingthestimulus)totheambiguoussignalsarerecorded.
Indogs,afoodbowlisplacedinonecornerofaroomandanemptybowlinanothercorner(Mendletal.
2010a).
Whendogslearntodiscriminatethetwolocations,abowlisplacedbetweenthem.
Inthetesttrials,approachtimetotheambiguouslocationsismeasured:aquickapproachindicatesanticipationoffood,an"optimistic"judgment,andaslowapproachindicatesa"pessimistic"judgment(Mendletal.
2010a).
Dogswithhigherseparation-relatedanxietyapproachtheambiguousbowllocationsmoreslowly,showinganegativecognitivebias(Mendletal.
2010a).
Anxiolyticmedicationwiththehumananti-depressantfluoxetinecombinedwithbehavioralmodificationdiminishesthebias(Karagiannisetal.
2015).
However,leavingdogsaloneforabrieftimedoesnotgeneratenegativeexpectations(Mülleretal.
2012),suggestingthatitisaprolongedemotionalstatethatinducesnegativebiasindogs(Mendletal.
2010a).
Likewise,brieflysearchingfortreatspriortotestingwasnotenoughtoinducepositivebiasindogs(Burmanetal.
2011),whereasadministeringoxytocinpriortotestingcausedpositivebias(Kisetal.
2015).
Todate,itseemsthatthecognitivebiastestmeasureslong-termtendencies,expectations,andmoodsratherthansuddenemotions.
Inducingthepositiveornegativeexpectationsexperimentallyhasproventricky,leavingroomforfurtherinvestigation.
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions1614.
Emotionalandsocialdataacrossspecies:EmotionalevolutionAlthoughdirectdataondogemotionsarecurrentlyquiterare,therearemorestudiesondogskillsininterspecificcooperative-communicativesocialtasks.
Inthesetasks,parallelexperimentsindogsandothercanids,carnivores,andnon-humanprimateshavebeenextremelyinformative.
Regardingevolutionarychangesinemotionleadingtochangesinsocialcognition,Hare(2007)hassuggestedthat"dogs'specializedsocial-problem-solvingskillsmayhavefirstappearedaftersystemsmediatingfearandaggressionwerealtered"(p.
62).
Inthelong-termstudiesonexperimentaldomesticationoffoxes,selectivebreedingforlowlevelsoffearandaggressiontowardhumanswereassociatedwithchangesinfoxes'limbicsystems(Trut2001),andasaside-effect,theirsocialcognitiveabilities(Hareetal.
2005).
ThisEmotionalReactivityHypothesis(Hare&Tomasello2005)wasrecentlytestedwithdogsandwolvesbyRange,Ritter,andViranyi(2015).
Inacooperativesituation,wolveswerenotmoreaggressivetowardsconspecificsthandogs.
Thus,themodifiedCanineCooperationHypothesisisthatdog-humancooperationmighthaveoriginatedfromwolf-wolftoleranceandcooperation(Range&Viranyi2014,Rangeetal.
2015).
Thesuggestionthatthereisinterplaybetweenemotionalandsocialskillsindogsisintriguing,andmeritsacloserexaminationofthebraincircuitriesinvolved.
15.
MethodologicaladvancesandfuturedirectionsHumanandnonhumanemotionshavealonghistoryofbeingstudiedusingdifferentmethods(Berridge2003).
Therefore,usingcomparablemethodsinhumansanddogsshouldprovidevaluablenewinsights(forexample,Raccaetal.
2012,Andicsetal.
2014,Trnqvistetal.
2015,Yong&Ruffman2016).
Methodologicaladvancesincludenon-invasivebrainimagingusuallyusedwithhumans,suchasfunctionalmagneticresonanceimaging(Bernsetal.
2012,Andicsetal.
2014,Jiaetal.
2014,Bernsetal.
2015,Dilksetal.
2015,Cuayaetal.
2016)andsurface-electroencephalography(Howelletal.
2012,Kujalaetal.
2013,Trnqvistetal.
2013,Kisetal.
2014).
Thesecannowbeusedwithdogstogetherwithpositiveoperant-conditionaltraining.
Thermographicimagingalsoappearspromisingfordetectingemotionally-stimulatedchangesinbodysurfacetemperature(Travainetal.
2015,Riemeretal.
2016,Travainetal.
2016).
Allthesenewtechniquesrequirecarefulexperimentationtoavoidthepossibleconfoundsreportedforhumanresearch(importantly,seeBennettetal.
2009,Kriegeskorteetal.
2009,Poldrack&Mumford2009,Vuletal.
2009).
Numeroustopics,suchastheoryofmindindogs,andpossibleemotionallateralizationorgendereffects,deservedmorediscussion.
Manypersonalandenvironmentalfactorsunderlieindividualdifferencesinhumanemotionalprocesses(e.
g.
,Tomarkenetal.
1992,Canlietal.
2002,deRosnay&Harris2002,Gross&John2003),andsimilarvariationalsooccursindogs(seee.
g.
,Goslingetal.
2003,Fratkinetal.
2013,Cooketal.
2014).
Dominancerelationsmayaffectcanineemotionsthroughcerebralneurochemicalconcentrations(forreview,Chichinadzeetal.
2014).
Skullshapeindogscanalsoaffectbrainformationandhencecognitionandemotion(McGreevyetal.
2004,Helton2009,Robertsetal.
2010,McGreevyetal.
2013).
Thereisalsonoreasondogscouldnothaveuniqueemotionalstatesthathumansdonothave,forexample,statesrelatedtotheirolfactoryworldandthefunctionofthepiriformcortex.
Thesetopicswillcertainlyreceivemoreattentioninthefuture.
WeneedAnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions17morestudiesoftheemotionalworldofdogsalongwithsensiblecautionininterpretingandgeneralizingtheresults.
AcknowledgmentsThisworkwasenabledbytheBrainResearchcollaborationbetweentheAaltoUniversityandUniversityofHelsinki(BRAHEinitiative);EmilAaltonenFoundationPersonalGrant(#160121);andTekesgrant#7244/31/2016.
Inadditiontotheeditorsandreviewers,Iwishtothankmycolleaguesfromvariousfieldsinthehumanandanimalsciencesfortheircommentsonanearlierdraftofthearticle:OttoLappi,SanniSomppi,HelenaTelknranta,RiittaHari,JariHietanen,JukkaLeppnen,OutiVainio,andKatriinaTiira.
ReferencesAdolphs,R.
(2002).
Recognizingemotionfromfacialexpressions:psychologicalandneurologicalmechanisms.
BehavioralandCognitiveNeuroscienceReviews,1,21–62.
doi:10.
1177/1534582302001001003Albuquerque,N.
,Guo,K.
,Wilkinson,A.
,Savalli,C.
,Otta,E.
&Mills,D.
(2016).
Dogsrecognizedogandhumanemotions.
BiologyLetters,12,20150883.
doi:10.
1098/rsbl.
2015.
0883Anderson,D.
J.
&Adolphs,R.
(2014).
Aframeworkforstudyingemotionsacrossspecies.
Cell,157,187-200.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cell.
2014.
03.
003Andics,A.
,Gácsi,M.
,Faragó,T.
,Kis,A.
&Miklósi,A.
(2014).
Voice-sensitiveregionsinthedogandhumanbrainarerevealedbycomparativefMRI.
CurrentBiology,24,574-578.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cub.
2014.
01.
058Ascione,F.
R.
(1992).
Enhancingchildrensattitudesaboutthehumanetreatmentofanimals:generalizationtohuman-directedempathy.
Anthrozos,5,176–191.
doi:10.
2752/089279392787011421Barber,A.
L.
,Randi,D.
,Müller,C.
A.
&Huber,L.
(2016).
Theprocessingofhumanemotionalfacesbypetandlabdogs:evidenceforlateralizationandexperienceeffects.
PlosONE,11,e0152393.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0152393Bartholomew,A.
J.
&Cirulli,E.
T.
(2014).
Individualvariationincontagiousyawningsusceptibilityishighlystableandlargelyunexplainedbyempathyorotherknownfactors.
PlosONE,9,e91773.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0091773Beaver,B.
V.
(1983).
Clinicalclassificationofcanineaggression.
AppliedAnimalEthology,10,35–43.
doi:10.
1016/0304-3762(83)90110-4CallforCommentary:AnimalSentiencepublishesOpenPeerCommentaryonallacceptedtargetarticles.
Targetarticlesarepeer-reviewed.
Commentariesareeditoriallyreviewed.
Therearesubmittedcommentariesaswellasinvitedcommentaries.
Commentariesappearassoonastheyhavebeenreviewed,revisedandaccepted.
Targetarticleauthorsmayrespondtotheircommentariesindividuallyorinajointresponsetomultiplecommentaries.
Instructions:http://animalstudiesrepository.
org/animsent/guidelines.
htmlAnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions18Beerda,B.
,Schilder,M.
B.
H.
,vanHooff,J.
A.
R.
A.
M.
,deVries,H.
W.
&Mol,J.
A.
(1998).
Behavioural,salivacortisolandheartrateresponsestodifferenttypesofstimuliindogs.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,58,365–381.
doi:10.
1016/S0168-1591(97)00145-7Bekoff,M.
(1974a).
Socialplayandplay-solicitingbyinfantcanids.
AmericanZoologist,14,323–340.
URL:http://www.
jstor.
org/stable/3881994Bekoff,M.
(1974b).
Socialplayincoyotes,wolves,anddogs.
Bioscience,24,225–230.
doi:10.
2307/1296803Bekoff,M.
(1995).
Playsignalsaspunctuation:thestructureofsocialplayincanids.
Behaviour,132,419–429.
doi:10.
1163/156853995x00649Bekoff,M.
(2001).
Observationsofscent-markinganddiscriminatingselffromothersbyadomesticdog(Canisfamiliaris):talesofdisplacedyellowsnow.
BehaviouralProcesses,55,75–79.
doi:10.
1016/S0376-6357(01)00142-5Bekoff,M.
(2007).
Theemotionallivesofanimals.
Novato,CA:NewWorldLibrary.
Ben-AmiBartal,I.
,Decety,J.
&Mason,P.
(2011).
Empathyandpro-socialbehaviorinrats.
Science,334,1427–1430.
doi:10.
1126/science.
1210789Bennett,C.
M.
,Miller,M.
&Wolford,G.
(2009).
Neuralcorrelatesofinter-speciesperspectivetakinginthepost-mortemAtlanticSalmon:anargumentformultiplecomparisonscorrection.
Neuroimage,47,S125.
doi:10.
1016/S1053-8119(09)71202-9Bernhardt,B.
C.
&Singer,T.
(2012).
Theneuralbasisofempathy.
AnnualreviewofNeuroscience,35,1–23.
doi:10.
1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150536Berns,G.
S.
,Brooks,A.
M.
&Spivak,M.
(2012).
FunctionalMRIinawakeunrestraineddogs.
PlosONE,7,e38027.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0038027Berns,G.
S.
,Brooks,A.
M.
&Spivak,M.
(2015).
Scentofthefamiliar:anfMRIstudyofcaninebrainresponsestofamiliarandunfamiliarhumananddogodors.
BehaviouralProcesses,110,37–46.
doi:10.
1016/j.
beproc.
2014.
02.
011Berridge,K.
C.
(2003).
Comparingtheemotionalbrainsofhumansandotheranimals.
InR.
J.
Davidson,K.
R.
Scherer&H.
H.
Goldsmith(Eds.
),Handbookofaffectivesciences(pp.
25–51).
NewYork:OxfordUniversityPressBirbaumer,N.
,Veit,R.
,Lotze,M.
,Erb,M.
,Hermann,C.
,Grodd,W.
&Flor,H.
(2005).
Deficientfearconditioninginpsychopathy:afunctionalmagneticresonanceimagingstudy.
ArchivesofGeneralPsychiatry,62,799–805.
doi:10.
1001/archpsyc.
62.
7.
799Blair,R.
J.
(2005).
Respondingtotheemotionsofothers:dissociatingformsofempathythroughthestudyoftypicalandpsychiatricpopulations.
ConsciousnessandCognition,14,698–718.
doi:10.
1016/j.
concog.
2005.
06.
004Blake,P.
R.
&McAuliffe,K.
(2011).
"Ihadsomuchitdidn'tseemfair":eight-year-oldsrejecttwoformsofinequity.
Cognition,120,215–224.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cognition.
2011.
04.
006Bloom,T.
&Friedman,H.
(2013).
Classifyingdogs'(Canisfamiliaris)facialexpressionsfromphotographs.
BehaviouralProcesses,96,1–10.
doi:10.
1016/j.
beproc.
2013.
02.
010Blythe,P.
W.
,Todd,P.
M.
&Miller,G.
F.
(1999).
Howmotionrevealsintention:categorizingsocialinteractions.
InG.
Gigerenzer,P.
M.
Todd&T.
A.
R.
Group(Eds.
),Simpleheuristicsthatmakeussmart(pp.
257–285).
NewYork:OxfordUniversityPressBolwig,N.
(1964).
Facialexpressioninprimateswithremarksonaparalleldevelopmentincertaincarnivores(Apreliminaryreportonworkinprogress).
Behaviour,22,167–192.
URL:http://www.
jstor.
org/stable/4533071AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions19Bower,G.
H.
(1991).
Moodcongruityofsocialjudgments.
InJ.
P.
Forgas(Ed.
),Emotionandsocialjudgments(pp.
31–53).
Oxford:PergamonPress.
Bruer,J.
(2015).
IdonotunderstandbutIcare:theprosocialdog.
InteractionStudies,16,341–360.
doi:10.
1075/is.
16.
3.
01braBrosnan,S.
F.
&DeWaal,F.
B.
(2003).
Monkeysrejectunequalpay.
Nature,425,297–299.
doi:10.
1038/nature01963Brosnan,S.
F.
,Schiff,H.
C.
&deWaal,F.
B.
(2005).
Toleranceforinequitymayincreasewithsocialclosenessinchimpanzees.
ProceedingsoftheRoyalSocietyB:BiologicalSciences,272,253–258.
doi:10.
1098/rspb.
2004.
2947Brosnan,S.
F.
,Talbot,C.
,Ahlgren,M.
,Lambeth,S.
P.
&Schapiro,S.
J.
(2010).
Mechanismsunderlyingresponsestoinequitableoutcomesinchimpanzees,Pantroglodytes.
AnimalBehaviour,79,1229–1237.
doi:10.
1016/j.
anbehav.
2010.
02.
019Brucks,D.
,Essler,J.
L.
,Marshall-Pescini,S.
&Range,F.
(2016).
Inequityaversionnegativelyaffectstoleranceandcontact-seekingBbehaviourstowardspartnerandexperimenter.
PlosONE,11,e0153799.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0153799Buckland,E.
L.
,Volk,H.
A.
,Burn,C.
C.
&Abeyesinghe,S.
M.
(2014).
Guardianperceptionsofcompaniondogexpressionsofpositiveemotionalstatesandthecontextsinwhichtheyoccur.
AnimalWelfare,23,287–296.
doi:10.
7120/09627286.
23.
3.
287Burman,O.
,McGowan,R.
,Mendl,M.
,Norling,Y.
,Paul,E.
,Rehn,T.
&Keeling,L.
(2011).
Usingjudgementbiastomeasurepositiveaffectivestateindogs.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,132,160–168.
doi:10.
1016/j.
applanim.
2011.
04.
001Burnett,S.
&Blakemore,S.
J.
(2009).
Functionalconnectivityduringasocialemotiontaskinadolescentsandinadults.
EuropeanJournalofNeuroscience,29,1294–1301.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1460-9568.
2009.
06674.
xBush,G.
,Luu,P.
&Posner,M.
I.
(2000).
Cognitiveandemotionalinfluencesinanteriorcingulatecortex.
TrendsinCognitiveSciences,4,215–222.
doi:10.
1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2Buttelmann,D.
&Tomasello,M.
(2013).
Candomesticdogs(Canisfamiliaris)usereferentialemotionalexpressionstolocatehiddenfoodAnimalCognition,16,137–145.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-012-0560-4Buttner,A.
P.
&Strasser,R.
(2014).
Contagiousyawning,socialcognition,andarousal:aninvestigationoftheprocessesunderlyingshelterdogs'responsestohumanyawns.
AnimalCognition,17,95–104.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-013-0641-zCaeiro,C.
C.
,Waller,B.
M.
,Burrows,A.
M.
,Zimmermann,E.
&Davila-Ross,M.
(2013).
OrangFACS:amuscle-basedcodingsystemfororangutanfacialmovements.
InternationalJournalofPrimatology,34,115–129.
doi:10.
1007/s10764-012-9652-xCalder,A.
J.
&Young,A.
W.
(2005).
Understandingtherecognitionoffacialidentityandfacialexpression.
NatureReviewsNeuroscience,6,641–651.
doi:10.
1038/nrn1724Call,J.
,Bruer,J.
,Kaminski,J.
&Tomasello,M.
(2003).
Domesticdogs(Canisfamiliaris)aresensitivetotheattentionalstateofhumans.
JournalofComparativePsychology,117,257–263.
doi:10.
1037/0735-7036.
117.
3.
257Cameron,D.
B.
(1997).
Caninedominance-associatedaggression:concepts,incidence,andtreatmentinaprivatebehaviorpractice.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,52,265–274.
doi:10.
1016/S0168-1591(96)01127-6AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions20Canli,T.
,Sivers,H.
,Whitfield,S.
L.
,Gotlib,I.
H.
&Gabrieli,J.
D.
(2002).
Amygdalaresponsetohappyfacesasafunctionofextraversion.
Science,296,2191.
doi:10.
1126/science.
1068749Chaminade,T.
,Hodgins,J.
&Kawato,M.
(2007).
Anthropomorphisminfluencesperceptionofcomputer-animatedcharacters'actions.
SocialCognitive&AffectiveNeuroscience,2,206–216.
doi:10.
1093/scan/nsm017Chichinadze,K.
,Chichinadze,N.
,Gachechiladze,L.
,Lazarashvili,A.
&Nikolaishvili,M.
(2014).
Physicalpredictors,behavioural/emotionalattributesandneurochemicaldeterminantsofdominantbehaviour.
BiologicalReviewsoftheCambridgePhilosophicalSociety,89,1005–1020.
doi:10.
1111/brv.
12091Church,R.
M.
(1959).
Emotionalreactionsofratstothepainofothers.
JournalofComparativeandPhysiologicalPsychology,52,132–134.
doi:10.
1037/h0043531Cook,P.
F.
,Spivak,M.
&Berns,G.
S.
(2014).
Onepairofhandsisnotlikeanother:caudateBOLDresponseindogsdependsonsignalsourceandcaninetemperament.
PeerJ,2,e596.
doi:10.
7717/peerj.
596Cools,A.
K.
A.
,VanHout,A.
J.
M.
&Nelissen,M.
H.
J.
(2007).
Caninereconciliationandthird-party-initiatedpostconflictaffiliation:DopeacemakingsocialmechanismsindogsrivalthoseofhigherprimatesEthology,114,53–63.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1439-0310.
2007.
01443.
xCordoni,G.
&Palagi,E.
(2015).
Beingavictimoranaggressor:differentfunctionsoftriadicpost-conflictinteractionsinwolves(Canislupuslupus).
AggressiveBehavior,41,526–536.
doi:10.
1002/ab.
21590Cuaya,L.
V.
,Hernandez-Perez,R.
&Concha,L.
(2016).
Ourfacesinthedog'sbrain:Functionalimagingrevealstemporalcortexactivationduringperceptionofhumanfaces.
PlosONE,11,e0149431.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0149431Cullen,H.
,Kanai,R.
,Bahrami,B.
&Rees,G.
(2014).
Individualdifferencesinanthropomorphicattributionsandhumanbrainstructure.
SocialCognitiveandAffectiveNeuroscience,9,1276–1280.
doi:10.
1093/scan/nst109Dadds,M.
R.
,Hunter,K.
,Hawes,D.
J.
,Frost,A.
D.
,Vassallo,S.
,Bunn,P.
,Merz,S.
&Masry,Y.
E.
(2008).
Ameasureofcognitiveandaffectiveempathyinchildrenusingparentratings.
ChildPsychiatry&HumanDevelopment,39,111–122.
doi:10.
1007/s10578-007-0075-4Damasio,A.
R.
,Grabowski,T.
J.
,Bechara,A.
,Damasio,H.
,Ponto,L.
L.
,Parvizi,J.
&Hichwa,R.
D.
(2000).
Subcorticalandcorticalbrainactivityduringthefeelingofself-generatedemotions.
NatureNeuroscience,3,1049–1056.
doi:10.
1038/79871Darwin,C.
(1872).
Theexpressionoftheemotionsinmanandanimals.
London:JohnMurrayDatta,R.
,Lee,J.
,Duda,J.
,Avants,B.
B.
,Vite,C.
H.
,Tseng,B.
,Gee,J.
C.
,Aguirre,G.
D.
&Aguirre,G.
K.
(2012).
Adigitalatlasofthedogbrain.
PlosONE,7,e52140.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0052140Davis,M.
H.
(1980).
Amultidimensionalapproachtoindividualdifferencesinempathy.
JSASCatalogofSelectedDocumentsinPsychology,10,85.
deLahunta,A.
&Glass,E.
(2009).
Veterinaryneuroanatomyandclinicalneurology.
St.
Louis,MO:Saunders,ElsevierInc.
deRosnay,M.
&Harris,P.
L.
(2002).
Individualdifferencesinchildren'sunderstandingofemotion:therolesofattachmentandlanguage.
Attachment&HumanDevelopment,4,39–54.
doi:10.
1080/14616730210123139AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions21deWaal,F.
B.
&Ferrari,P.
F.
(2010).
Towardsabottom-upperspectiveonanimalandhumancognition.
TrendsinCognitiveSciences,14,201–207.
doi:10.
1016/j.
tics.
2010.
03.
003deWaal,F.
B.
&Suchak,M.
(2010).
Prosocialprimates:selfishandunselfishmotivations.
PhilosophicalTransactionsoftheRoyalSocietyB:BiologicalSciences,365,2711–2722.
doi:10.
1098/rstb.
2010.
0119deWaal,F.
B.
&vanRoosmalen,A.
(1979).
Reconciliationandconsolationamongchimpanzees.
BehavioralEcologyandSociobiology,5,55–66.
URL:http://www.
jstor.
org/stable/4599217Decety,J.
&Jackson,P.
L.
(2004).
Thefunctionalarchitectureofhumanempathy.
BehavioralandCognitiveNeuroscienceReviews,3,71–100.
doi:10.
1177/1534582304267187Dilks,D.
D.
,Cook,P.
,Weiller,S.
K.
,Berns,H.
P.
,Spivak,M.
&Berns,G.
S.
(2015).
AwakefMRIrevealsaspecializedregionindogtemporalcortexforfaceprocessing.
PeerJ,3,e1115.
doi:10.
7717/peerj.
1115Dreschel,N.
A.
&Granger,D.
A.
(2005).
Physiologicalandbehavioralreactivitytostressinthunderstorm-phobicdogsandtheircaregivers.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,95,153–168.
doi:10.
1016/j.
applanim.
2005.
04.
009Duffy,D.
L.
,Hsu,Y.
Y.
&Serpell,J.
A.
(2008).
Breeddifferencesincanineaggression.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,114,441–460.
doi:10.
1016/j.
applanim.
2008.
04.
006Eisenberg,N.
&Fabes,R.
A.
(1990).
Empathy:conceptualization,measurement,andrelationtoprosocialbehavior.
MotivationandEmotion,14,131–149.
doi:10.
1007/BF00991640Eisenberg,N.
,Guthrie,I.
K.
,Murphy,B.
C.
,Shepard,S.
A.
,Cumberland,A.
&Carlo,G.
(1999).
Consistencyanddevelopmentofprosocialdispositions:alongitudinalstudy.
ChildDevelopment,70,1360–1372.
doi:10.
1111/1467-8624.
00100Eisenberg,N.
&Miller,P.
A.
(1987).
Therelationofempathytoprosocialandrelatedbehaviors.
PsychologicalBulletin,101,91–119.
doi:10.
1037/0033-2909.
101.
1.
91Ekman,P.
(1992).
AretherebasicemotionsPsychologicalReview,99,550–553.
doi:10.
1037/0033-295X.
99.
3.
550Ekman,P.
&Friesen,W.
V.
(1971).
Constantsacrossculturesinthefaceandemotion.
JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,17,124–129.
doi:10.
1037/h0030377Ekman,P.
&Friesen,W.
V.
(1978).
Facialactioncodingsystem.
CA:ConsultingPsychologyPress.
Ekman,P.
,Friesen,W.
V.
,Osullivan,M.
,Chan,A.
,Diacoyannitarlatzis,I.
,Heider,K.
,Krause,R.
,Lecompte,W.
A.
,Pitcairn,T.
,Riccibitti,P.
E.
,Scherer,K.
,Tomita,M.
&Tzavaras,A.
(1987).
Universalsandcultural-differencesinthejudgmentsoffacialexpressionsofemotion.
JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,53,712–717.
doi:10.
1037/0022-3514.
53.
4.
712Elfenbein,H.
A.
&Ambady,N.
(2002).
Ontheuniversalityandculturalspecificityofemotionrecognition:ameta-analysis.
PsychologicalBulletin,128,203–235.
doi:10.
1037//0033-2909.
128.
2.
203Epley,N.
,Waytz,A.
&Cacioppo,J.
T.
(2007).
Onseeinghuman:athree-factortheoryofanthropomorphism.
PsychologicalReview,114,864–886.
doi:10.
1037/0033-295X.
114.
4.
864AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions22Etkin,A.
,Egner,T.
&Kalisch,R.
(2011).
Emotionalprocessinginanteriorcingulateandmedialprefrontalcortex.
TrendsinCognitiveSciences,15,85–93.
doi:10.
1016/j.
tics.
2010.
11.
004Evans,H.
E.
&deLahunta,A.
(2013).
Miller'sanatomyofthedog.
St.
Louis,MO:Saunders,ElsevierInc.
Eysenck,M.
W.
,Mogg,K.
,May,J.
,Richards,A.
&Mathews,A.
(1991).
Biasininterpretationofambiguoussentencesrelatedtothreatinanxiety.
JournalofAbnormalPsychology,100,144–150.
doi:10.
1037/0021-843X.
100.
2.
144Faragó,T.
,Andics,A.
,Devecseri,V.
,Kis,A.
,Gácsi,M.
&Miklósi,A.
(2014).
Humansrelyonthesamerulestoassessemotionalvalenceandintensityinconspecificanddogvocalizations.
BiologyLetters,10,20130926.
doi:10.
1098/rsbl.
2013.
0926Fazio,R.
H.
(2010).
Ontheautomaticactivationofassociatedevaluations:anoverview.
CognitionandEmotion,15,115–141.
doi:10.
1080/02699930125908Fehr,E.
&Camerer,C.
F.
(2007).
Socialneuroeconomics:theneuralcircuitryofsocialpreferences.
TrendsinCognitiveSciences,11,419–427.
doi:10.
1016/j.
tics.
2007.
09.
002Fehr,E.
&Rockenbach,B.
(2004).
Humanaltruism:economic,neural,andevolutionaryperspectives.
CurrentOpinioninNeurobiology,14,784–790.
doi:10.
1016/j.
conb.
2004.
10.
007Fox,M.
W.
(1970).
Acomparativestudyofdevelopmentoffacialexpressionsincanids:wolf,coyoteandfoxes.
Behaviour,36,49–73.
doi:10.
1163/156853970x00042Franklin,R.
G.
,Jr.
,Nelson,A.
J.
,Baker,M.
,Beeney,J.
E.
,Vescio,T.
K.
,Lenz-Watson,A.
&Adams,R.
B.
,Jr.
(2013).
Neuralresponsestoperceivingsufferinginhumansandanimals.
SocialNeuroscience,8,217–227.
doi:10.
1080/17470919.
2013.
763852Fratkin,J.
L.
,Sinn,D.
L.
,Patall,E.
A.
&Gosling,S.
D.
(2013).
Personalityconsistencyindogs:ameta-analysis.
PlosONE,8,e54907.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0054907Gallup,G.
G.
,Jr.
&Suarez,S.
D.
(1986).
Self-awarenessandtheemergenceofmindinhumansandotherprimates.
InJ.
Suls&A.
Greenwald(Eds.
),Psychologicalperspectivesontheself(Vol.
3)(pp.
3–26).
Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.
Galvan,M.
&Vonk,J.
(2016).
Man'sotherbestfriend:domesticcats(F.
silvestriscatus)andtheirdiscriminationofhumanemotioncues.
AnimalCognition,19,193–205.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-015-0927-4Gazzola,V.
,Aziz-Zadeh,L.
&Keysers,C.
(2006).
Empathyandthesomatotopicauditorymirrorsysteminhumans.
CurrentBiology,16,1824–1829.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cub.
2006.
07.
072Gazzola,V.
,Rizzolatti,G.
,Wicker,B.
&Keysers,C.
(2007).
Theanthropomorphicbrain:themirrorneuronsystemrespondstohumanandroboticactions.
Neuroimage,35,1674–1684.
doi:10.
1016/j.
neuroimage.
2007.
02.
003Giganti,F.
&EspositoZiello,M.
(2009).
Contagiousandspontaneousyawninginautisticandtypicallydevelopingchildren.
CurrentPsychologyLetters,25,1–11.
Goddard,M.
E.
&Beilharz,R.
G.
(1986).
Earlypredictionofadultbehaviorinpotentialguidedogs.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,15,247–260.
doi:10.
1016/0168-1591(86)90095-XGosling,S.
D.
,Kwan,V.
S.
&John,O.
P.
(2003).
Adog'sgotpersonality:across-speciescomparativeapproachtopersonalityjudgmentsindogsandhumans.
JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,85,1161–1169.
doi:10.
1037/0022-3514.
85.
6.
1161AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions23Gotlib,I.
H.
&Krasnoperova,E.
(1998).
Biasedinformationprocessingasavulnerabilityfactorfordepression.
BehaviorTherapy,29,603–617.
doi:10.
1016/S0005-7894(98)80020-8Gross,J.
J.
&John,O.
P.
(2003).
Individualdifferencesintwoemotionregulationprocesses:implicationsforaffect,relationships,andwell-being.
JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,85,348–362.
doi:10.
1037/0022-3514.
85.
2.
348Handlin,L.
,Hydbring-Sandberg,E.
,Nilsson,A.
,Ejdeback,M.
,Jansson,A.
&Uvnas-Moberg,K.
(2011).
Short-terminteractionbetweendogsandtheirguardians:effectsonoxytocin,cortisol,insulinandheartrate:anexploratorystudy.
Anthrozos,24,301–315.
doi:10.
2752/175303711x13045914865385Harding,E.
J.
,Paul,E.
S.
&Mendl,M.
(2004).
Animalbehaviour:cognitivebiasandaffectivestate.
Nature,427,312.
doi:10.
1038/427312aHare,B.
(2007).
Fromnonhumantohumanmind:whatchangedandwhyCurrentDirectionsinPsychologicalScience,16,60–64.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1467-8721.
2007.
00476.
xHare,B.
&Tomasello,M.
(2005).
Theemotionalreactivityhypothesisandcognitiveevolution:ReplytoMiklósiandTopal.
TrendsinCognitiveSciences,9,464–465.
doi:10.
1016/j.
tics.
2005.
08.
010Hare,B.
,Call,J.
&Tomasello,M.
(1998).
Communicationoffoodlocationbetweenhumananddog(Canisfamiliaris).
EvolutionofCommunication,2,137–159.
doi:10.
1075/eoc.
2.
1.
06harHare,B.
,Plyusnina,I.
,Ignacio,N.
,Schepina,O.
,Stepika,A.
,Wrangham,R.
&Trut,L.
(2005).
Socialcognitiveevolutionincaptivefoxesisacorrelatedby-productofexperimentaldomestication.
CurrentBiology,15,226–230.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cub.
2005.
01.
040Hari,R.
&Kujala,M.
V.
(2009).
Brainbasisofhumansocialinteraction:fromconceptstobrainimaging.
PhysiologicalReviews,89,453–479.
doi:10.
1152/physrev.
00041.
2007Harris,C.
R.
&Prouvost,C.
(2014).
Jealousyindogs.
PlosONE,9,e94597.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0094597Harris,L.
T.
&Fiske,S.
T.
(2006).
Dehumanizingthelowestofthelow:neuroimagingresponsestoextremeout-groups.
PsychologicalScience,17,847–853.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1467-9280.
2006.
01793.
xHarrison,M.
A.
&Hall,A.
E.
(2010).
Anthropomorphism,empathy,andperceivedcommunicativeabilityvarywithphylogeneticrelatednesstohumans.
JournalofSocialEvolutionaryCulturalPsychology,4,34–48.
doi:10.
1037/h0099303Hart,A.
J.
,Whalen,P.
J.
,Shin,L.
M.
,McInerney,S.
C.
,Fischer,H.
&Rauch,S.
L.
(2000).
Differentialresponseinthehumanamygdalatoracialoutgroupvs.
ingroupfacestimuli.
Neuroreport,11,2351–2355.
Hart,S.
&Carrington,H.
A.
(2002).
Jealousyinsix-month-oldinfants.
Infancy,3,395–402.
doi:10.
1207/S15327078IN0303_6Hart,S.
,Field,T.
,DelValle,C.
&Letourneau,M.
(1998).
Infantsprotesttheirmothers'attendingtoaninfant-sizedoll.
SocialDevelopment,7,54–61.
doi:10.
1111/1467-9507.
00050Haselton,M.
G.
,Nettle,D.
&Andrews,P.
W.
(2005).
Theevolutionofcognitivebias.
InD.
M.
Buss(Ed.
),Thehandbookofevolutionarypsychology(pp.
724–746).
Hoboken,NJ:JohnWiley&SonsInc.
AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions24Haslam,N.
(2006).
Dehumanization:anintegrativereview.
PersonalityandSocialPsychologyReview,10,252–264.
doi:10.
1207/s15327957pspr1003_4Haug,L.
I.
(2008).
Canineaggressiontowardunfamiliarpeopleanddogs.
VeterinaryClinicsofNorthAmerica-SmallAnimalPractice,38,1023–1041.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cvsm.
2008.
04.
005Haxby,J.
V.
,Hoffman,E.
A.
&Gobbini,M.
I.
(2000).
Thedistributedhumanneuralsystemforfaceperception.
TrendsinCognitiveSciences,4,223–233.
doi:10.
1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0Hebb,D.
O.
(1955).
Themammalandhisenvironment.
AmericanJournalofPsychiatry,111,826–831.
doi:10.
1176/ajp.
111.
11.
826Hecht,J.
,Miklósi,A.
&Gácsi,M.
(2012).
Behavioralassessmentandguardianperceptionsofbehaviorsassociatedwithguiltindogs.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,139,134–142.
doi:10.
1016/j.
applanim.
2012.
02.
015Helt,M.
S.
,Eigsti,I.
M.
,Snyder,P.
J.
&Fein,D.
A.
(2010).
Contagiousyawninginautisticandtypicaldevelopment.
ChildDevelopment,81,1620–1631.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1467-8624.
2010.
01495.
xHelton,W.
S.
(2009).
Cephalicindexandperceiveddogtrainability.
BehaviouralProcesses,82,355–358.
doi:10.
1016/j.
beproc.
2009.
08.
004Horowitz,A.
(2009a).
Attentiontoattentionindomesticdog(Canisfamiliaris)dyadicplay.
AnimalCognition,12,107–118.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-008-0175-yHorowitz,A.
(2009b).
Disambiguatingthe"guiltylook":salientpromptstoafamiliardogbehaviour.
BehavouralProcesses,81,447–452.
doi:10.
1016/j.
beproc.
2009.
03.
014Horowitz,A.
(2011).
Theoryofmindindogs:examiningmethodandconcept.
Learning&Behavior,39,314–317.
doi:10.
3758/s13420-011-0041-7Horowitz,A.
(2012).
Fairisfine,butmoreisbetter:limitstoinequityaversioninthedomesticdog.
SocialJusticeResearch,25,195–212.
doi:10.
1007/s11211-012-0158-7Horvath,Z.
,Doka,A.
&Miklósi,A.
(2008).
Affiliativeanddisciplinarybehaviorofhumanhandlersduringplaywiththeirdogaffectscortisolconcentrationsinoppositedirections.
HormonesandBehavior,54,107–114.
doi:10.
1016/j.
yhbeh.
2008.
02.
002Howell,T.
J.
,Conduit,R.
,Toukhsati,S.
&Bennett,P.
(2012).
Auditorystimulusdiscriminationrecordedindogs,asindicatedbymismatchnegativity(MMN).
BehaviouralProcesses,89,8–13.
doi:10.
1016/j.
beproc.
2011.
09.
009Hsu,Y.
Y.
&Serpell,J.
A.
(2003).
Developmentandvalidationofaquestionnaireformeasuringbehaviorandtemperamenttraitsinpetdogs.
JournaloftheAmericanVeterinaryMedicalAssociation,223,1293–1300.
doi:10.
2460/javma.
2003.
223.
1293Hydbring-Sandberg,E.
,vonWalter,L.
W.
,Hoglund,K.
,Svartberg,K.
,Swenson,L.
&Forkman,B.
(2004).
Physiologicalreactionstofearprovocationindogs.
JournalofEndocrinology,180,439–448.
doi:10.
1677/joe.
0.
1800439Imamura,Y.
,Terada,K.
&Takahashi,H.
(2015).
Effectsofbehavioralcomplexityonintentionattributiontorobots.
Proceedingsofthe3rdInternationalConferenceonHuman-AgentInteraction,65–72.
NewYork,NY:ACM.
doi:10.
1145/2814940.
2814949Izard,C.
E.
(1992).
Basicemotions,relationsamongemotions,andemotion-cognitionrelations.
PsychologicalReview,99,561–565.
doi:10.
1037/0033-295X.
99.
3.
561AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions25Jackson,P.
L.
,Meltzoff,A.
N.
&Decety,J.
(2005).
HowdoweperceivethepainofothersAwindowintotheneuralprocessesinvolvedinempathy.
Neuroimage,24,771–779.
doi:10.
1016/j.
neuroimage.
2004.
09.
006Jensen,P.
(2007).
Mechanismsandfunctionindogbehaviour.
InP.
Jensen(Ed.
),Thebehaviouralbiologyofdogs(pp.
61–75).
Wallingford:CABI.
Jia,H.
,Pustovyy,O.
M.
,Waggoner,P.
,Beyers,R.
J.
,Schumacher,J.
,Wildey,C.
,Barrett,J.
,Morrison,E.
,Salibi,N.
,Denney,T.
S.
,Vodyanoy,V.
J.
&Deshpande,G.
(2014).
FunctionalMRIoftheolfactorysysteminconsciousdogs.
PlosONE,9,e86362.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0086362Johnson,M.
H.
,Dziurawiec,S.
,Ellis,H.
&Morton,J.
(1991).
Newborns'preferentialtrackingofface-likestimulianditssubsequentdecline.
Cognition,40,1–19.
doi:10.
1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6Joly-Mascheroni,R.
M.
,Senju,A.
&Shepherd,A.
J.
(2008).
Dogscatchhumanyawns.
BiologyLetters,4,446–448.
doi:10.
1098/rsbl.
2008.
0333Kaminski,J.
,Tempelmann,S.
,Call,J.
&Tomasello,M.
(2009).
Domesticdogscomprehendhumancommunicationwithiconicsigns.
DevelopmentalScience,12,831–837.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1467-7687.
2009.
00815.
xKanwisher,N.
,McDermott,J.
&Chun,M.
M.
(1997).
Thefusiformfacearea:amoduleinhumanextrastriatecortexspecializedforfaceperception.
JournalofNeuroscience,17,4302–4311.
Karagiannis,C.
I.
,Burman,O.
H.
P.
&Mills,D.
S.
(2015).
Dogswithseparation-relatedproblemsshowa"lesspessimistic"cognitivebiasduringtreatmentwithfluoxetine(Reconcile(TM))andabehaviourmodificationplan.
BMCVeterinaryResearch,11,80.
doi:10.
1186/s12917-015-0373-1Kendrick,K.
M.
&Baldwin,B.
A.
(1987).
Cellsintemporalcortexofconscioussheepcanrespondpreferentiallytothesightoffaces.
Science,236,448–450.
doi:10.
1126/science.
3563521Kiesler,S.
,Lee,S.
-L.
&Kramer,A.
D.
I.
(2006).
Relationshipeffectsinpsychologicalexplanationsofnonhumanbehavior.
Anthrozos,19,335–352.
doi:10.
2752/089279306785415448Kiesler,T.
&Kiesler,S.
(2005).
Mypetrockandme:anexperimentalexplorationoftheselfextensionconcept.
InG.
Menon&A.
R.
Rao(Eds.
),Advancesinconsumerresearch(pp.
365–370).
Duluth,MN:AssociationforConsumerResearch.
King,A.
S.
(1987).
Physiologicalandclinicalanatomyofthedomesticmammals.
Volume1:Centralnervoussystem.
Oxford:OxfordUniversityPress.
King,T.
,Hemsworth,P.
H.
&Coleman,G.
J.
(2003).
Fearofnovelandstartlingstimuliindomesticdogs.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,82,45–64.
doi:10.
1016/S0168-1591(03)00040-6Kis,A.
,Hernadi,A.
,Kanizsar,O.
,Gácsi,M.
&Topal,J.
(2015).
Oxytocininducespositiveexpectationsaboutambivalentstimuli(cognitivebias)indogs.
HormonesandBehavior,69,1–7.
doi:10.
1016/j.
yhbeh.
2014.
12.
004Kis,A.
,Szakadat,S.
,Kovacs,E.
,Gácsi,M.
,Simor,P.
,Gombos,F.
,Topal,J.
,Miklósi,A.
&Bodizs,R.
(2014).
Developmentofanon-invasivepolysomnographytechniquefordogs(Canisfamiliaris).
Physiology&Behavior,130,149–156.
doi:10.
1016/j.
physbeh.
2014.
04.
004AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions26Kriegeskorte,N.
,Simmons,W.
K.
,Bellgowan,P.
S.
&Baker,C.
I.
(2009).
Circularanalysisinsystemsneuroscience:thedangersofdoubledipping.
NatureNeuroscience,12,535–540.
doi:10.
1038/nn.
2303Kujala,M.
V.
,Kujala,J.
,Carlson,S.
&Hari,R.
(2012).
Dogexperts'brainsdistinguishsociallyrelevantbodyposturessimilarlyindogsandhumans.
PlosONE,7,e39145.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0039145Kujala,M.
V.
,Somppi,S.
,Jokela,M.
,Vainio,O.
&Parkkonen,L.
(2017).
Humanempathy,personalityandexperienceaffecttheemotionratingsofdogandhumanfacialexpressions.
PlosONE,12,e0170730.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0170730Kujala,M.
V.
,Trnqvist,H.
,Somppi,S.
,Hnninen,L.
,Krause,C.
M.
,Vainio,O.
&Kujala,J.
(2013).
Reactivityofdogs'brainoscillationstovisualstimulimeasuredwithnon-invasiveelectroencephalography.
PlosONE,8,e61818.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0061818Kwan,V.
S.
Y.
,Gosling,S.
D.
&John,O.
P.
(2008).
Anthropomorphismasaspecialcaseofsocialperception:Across-speciessocialrelationsmodelanalysisofhumansanddogs.
SocialCognition,26,129–142.
doi:10.
1521/soco.
2008.
26.
2.
129Lakestani,N.
N.
,Donaldson,M.
&Waran,N.
(2014).
Interpretationofdogbehaviourbychildrenandyoungadults.
Anthrozos,27,65–80.
doi:10.
2752/175303714x13837396326413Lawrence,E.
J.
,Shaw,P.
,Baker,D.
,Baron-Cohen,S.
&David,A.
S.
(2004).
Measuringempathy:reliabilityandvalidityoftheEmpathyQuotient.
PsychologicalMedicine,34,911–919.
doi:10.
1017/S0033291703001624Leary,M.
R.
(2003).
Theselfandemotion:theroleofself-reflectioninthegenerationandregulationofaffectiveexperience.
InR.
J.
Davidson,K.
R.
Scherer&H.
H.
Goldsmith(Eds.
),Handbookofaffectivesciences(pp.
773–786).
NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.
Lempers,J.
D.
,Flavell,E.
R.
&Flavell,J.
H.
(1977).
Developmentinveryyoungchildrenoftacitknowledgeconcerningvisualperception.
GeneticPsychologyMonographs,95,3–53.
Leopold,D.
A.
&Rhodes,G.
(2010).
Acomparativeviewoffaceperception.
JournalofComparativePsychology,124,233–251.
doi:10.
1037/a0019460Leppnen,J.
M.
&Nelson,C.
A.
(2009).
Tuningthedevelopingbraintosocialsignalsofemotions.
NatureReviewsNeuroscience,10,37–47.
doi:10.
1038/nrn2554Leyens,J.
-P.
,Paladino,P.
M.
,Rodriguez-Torres,R.
,Vaes,J.
,Demoulin,S.
,Rodriguez-Perez,A.
&Gaunt,R.
(2000).
Theemotionalsideofprejudice:theattributionofsecondaryemotionstoingroupsandoutgroups.
PersonalityandSocialPsychologyReview,4,186–197.
doi:10.
1207/S15327957PSPR0402_06MacLeod,A.
K.
&Byrne,A.
(1996).
Anxiety,depression,andtheanticipationoffuturepositiveandnegativeexperiences.
JournalofAbnormalPsychology,105,286–289.
doi:10.
1037/0021-843x.
105.
2.
286Madsen,E.
A.
&Persson,T.
(2013).
Contagiousyawningindomesticdogpuppies(Canislupusfamiliaris):theeffectofontogenyandemotionalclosenessonlow-levelimitationindogs.
AnimalCognition,16,233–240.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-012-0568-9Martini,M.
C.
,Gonzalez,C.
A.
&Wiese,E.
(2016).
Seeingmindsinothers:canagentswithroboticappearancehavehuman-likepreferencesPlosONE,11,e0146310.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0146310AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions27Masserman,J.
H.
,Wechkin,S.
&Terris,W.
(1964).
"Altruistic"behaviorinrhesusmonkeys.
TheAmericanJournalofPsychiatry,121,584–585.
doi:10.
1176/ajp.
121.
6.
584McGreevy,P.
D.
,Georgevsky,D.
,Carrasco,J.
,Valenzuela,M.
,Duffy,D.
L.
&Serpell,J.
A.
(2013).
Dogbehaviorco-varieswithheight,bodyweightandskullshape.
PlosONE,8,e80529.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0080529McGreevy,P.
,Grassi,T.
D.
&Harman,A.
M.
(2004).
Astrongcorrelationexistsbetweenthedistributionofretinalganglioncellsandnoselengthinthedog.
Brain,BehaviorandEvolution,63,13–22.
doi:10.
1159/000073756Mehrabian,A.
&Epstein,N.
(1972).
Measureofemotionalempathy.
JournalofPersonality,40,525–543.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1467-6494.
1972.
tb00078.
xMehrkam,L.
R.
&Wynne,C.
D.
L.
(2014).
Behavioraldifferencesamongbreedsofdomesticdogs(Canislupusfamiliaris):currentstatusofthescience.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,155,12–27.
doi:10.
1016/j.
applanim.
2014.
03.
005Meints,K.
,Racca,A.
&Hickey,N.
(2010).
HowtopreventdogbiteinjuriesChildrenmisinterpretdogsfacialexpressions.
InjuryPrevention,16,A68.
doi:10.
1136/ip.
2010.
029215.
0246Melzack,R.
(1952).
Irrationalfearsinthedog.
CanadianJournalofPsychology,6,141–147.
doi:10.
1037/H0083562Melzack,R.
(1954).
Thegenesisofemotionalbehavior;anexperimentalstudyofthedog.
JournalofComparativeandPhysiologicalPsychology,47,166–168.
doi:10.
1037/h0055840Mendl,M.
,Brooks,J.
,Basse,C.
,Burman,O.
,Paul,E.
,Blackwell,E.
&Casey,R.
(2010a).
Dogsshowingseparation-relatedbehaviourexhibita'pessimistic'cognitivebias.
CurrentBiology,20,R839–R840.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cub.
2010.
08.
030Mendl,M.
,Burman,O.
H.
&Paul,E.
S.
(2010b).
Anintegrativeandfunctionalframeworkforthestudyofanimalemotionandmood.
ProceedingsoftheRoyalSocietyB:BiologicalSciences,277,2895–2904.
doi:10.
1098/rspb.
2010.
0303Merola,I.
,Prato-Previde,E.
&Marshall-Pescini,S.
(2012a).
Dogs'socialreferencingtowardsguardiansandstrangers.
PlosONE,7,e47653.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0047653Merola,I.
,Prato-Previde,E.
&Marshall-Pescini,S.
(2012b).
Socialreferencingindog-guardiandyadsAnimalCognition,15,175–185.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-011-0443-0Merola,I.
,Prato-Previde,E.
,Lazzaroni,M.
&Marshall-Pescini,S.
(2014).
Dogs'comprehensionofreferentialemotionalexpressions:familiarpeopleandfamiliaremotionsareeasier.
AnimalCognition,17,373–385.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-013-0668-1Meyer,I.
&Forkman,B.
(2014).
Nonverbalcommunicationandhuman-doginteraction.
Anthrozos,27,553–568.
doi:10.
2752/089279314x14072268687925Meyer,I.
,Forkman,B.
&Paul,E.
S.
(2014).
Factorsaffectingthehumaninterpretationofdogbehavior.
Anthrozos,27,127–140.
doi:10.
2752/175303714x13837396326576Miklósi,A.
,Kubinyi,E.
,Topal,J.
,Gácsi,M.
,Viranyi,Z.
&Csanyi,V.
(2003).
Asimplereasonforabigdifference:wolvesdonotlookbackathumans,butdogsdo.
CurrentBiology,13,763–766.
doi:10.
1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-XMiller,S.
C.
,Kennedy,C.
,Devoe,D.
,Hickey,M.
,Nelson,T.
&Kogan,L.
(2009).
Anexaminationofchangesinoxytocinlevelsinmenandwomenbeforeandafterinteractionwithabondeddog.
Anthrozos,22,31–42.
doi:10.
2752/175303708x390455AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions28Mirkó,E.
,Doká,A.
&Miklósi,A.
(2013).
Associationbetweensubjectiveratingandbehaviourcodingandtheroleofexperienceinmakingvideoassessmentsonthepersonalityofthedomesticdog(Canisfamiliaris).
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,149,45–54.
doi:10.
1016/j.
applanim.
2013.
10.
003Moll,J.
,deOliveira-Souza,R.
,Bramati,I.
E.
&Grafman,J.
(2002).
Functionalnetworksinemotionalmoralandnonmoralsocialjudgments.
Neuroimage,16,696–703.
doi:S1053811902911183Morris,P.
H.
,Doe,C.
&Godsell,E.
(2008).
Secondaryemotionsinnon-primatespeciesBehaviouralreportsandsubjectiveclaimsbyanimalguardians.
CognitionandEmotion,22,3–20.
doi:10.
1080/02699930701273716Morris,P.
H.
,Knight,S.
&Lesley,S.
(2012).
Beliefinanimalmind:DoesfamiliaritywithanimalsinfluencebeliefsaboutanimalemotionsSociety&Animals,20,211–224.
doi:10.
1163/15685306-12341234Morrow,M.
,Ottobre,J.
,Ottobre,A.
,Neville,P.
,St-Pierre,N.
,Dreschel,N.
&Pate,J.
L.
(2015).
Breed-dependentdifferencesintheonsetoffear-relatedavoidancebehaviorinpuppies.
JournalofVeterinaryBehavior-ClinicalApplicationsandResearch,10,286–294.
doi:10.
1016/j.
jveb.
2015.
03.
002Müller,C.
A.
,Riemer,S.
,Rosam,C.
M.
,Schosswender,J.
,Range,F.
&Huber,L.
(2012).
Briefguardianabsencedoesnotinducenegativejudgementbiasinpetdogs.
AnimalCognition,15,1031–1035.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-012-0526-6Müller,C.
A.
,Schmitt,K.
,Barber,A.
L.
&Huber,L.
(2015).
Dogscandiscriminateemotionalexpressionsofhumanfaces.
CurrentBiology,25,601–605.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cub.
2014.
12.
055Nagasawa,M.
,Kikusui,T.
,Onaka,T.
&Ohta,M.
(2009).
Dog'sgazeatitsguardianincreasesguardian'surinaryoxytocinduringsocialinteraction.
HormonesandBehavior,55,434–441.
doi:10.
1016/j.
yhbeh.
2008.
12.
002Nagasawa,M.
,Mitsui,S.
,En,S.
,Ohtani,N.
,Ohta,M.
,Sakuma,Y.
,Onaka,T.
,Mogi,K.
&Kikusui,T.
(2015).
Socialevolution.
Oxytocin-gazepositiveloopandthecoevolutionofhuman-dogbonds.
Science,348,333–336.
doi:10.
1126/science.
1261022Nagasawa,M.
,Murai,K.
,Mogi,K.
&Kikusui,T.
(2011).
Dogscandiscriminatehumansmilingfacesfromblankexpressions.
AnimalCognition,14,525–533.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-011-0386-5Nagel,T.
(1974).
WhatisitliketobeabatThePhilosophicalReview,83,435–450.
doi:10.
2307/2183914Nass,C.
,Steuer,J.
&Tauber,E.
(1994).
Computersaresocialactors.
ProceedingsoftheConferenceonHumanFactorsinComputingSystemsCHI'94,72–78.
NewYork,NY:ACM.
doi:10.
1145/191666.
191703Netto,W.
J.
&Planta,D.
J.
U.
(1997).
Behaviouraltestingforaggressioninthedomesticdog.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,52,243–263.
doi:10.
1016/S0168-1591(96)01126-4Norring,M.
,Wikman,I.
,Hokkanen,A.
H.
,Kujala,M.
V.
&Hnninen,L.
(2014).
Empathicveterinariansscorecattlepainhigher.
TheVeterinaryJournal,200,186–190.
doi:10.
1016/j.
tvjl.
2014.
02.
005O'Hara,S.
J.
&Reeve,A.
V.
(2011).
Atestoftheyawningcontagionandemotionalconnectednesshypothesisindogs,Canisfamiliaris.
AnimalBehaviour,81,335–340.
doi:10.
1016/j.
anbehav.
2010.
11.
005AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions29Odendaal,J.
S.
J.
&Meintjes,R.
A.
(2003).
Neurophysiologicalcorrelatesofaffiliativebehaviourbetweenhumansanddogs.
TheVeterinaryJournal,165,296–301.
doi:10.
1016/S1090-0233(02)00237-XOgata,N.
,Kikusui,T.
,Takeuchi,Y.
&Mori,Y.
(2006).
Objectivemeasurementoffear-associatedlearningindogs.
JournalofVeterinaryBehavior-ClinicalApplicationsandResearch,1,55–61.
doi:10.
1016/j.
jveb.
2006.
06.
002Overall,K.
L.
(2000).
Naturalanimalmodelsofhumanpsychiatricconditions:assessmentofmechanismandvalidity.
ProgressinNeuro-Psychopharmacology&BiologicalPsychiatry,24,727–776.
doi:10.
1016/S0278-5846(00)00104-4Palagi,E.
&Cordoni,G.
(2009).
Postconflictthird-partyaffiliationinCanislupus:DowolvessharesimilaritieswiththegreatapesAnimalBehaviour,78,979–986.
doi:10.
1016/j.
anbehav.
2009.
07.
017Palagi,E.
,Nicotra,V.
&Cordoni,G.
(2015).
Rapidmimicryandemotionalcontagionindomesticdogs.
RoyalSocietyOpenScience,2,150505.
doi:10.
1098/rsos.
150505Palazzi,X.
(2011).
Thebeaglebraininstereotaxiccoordinates.
NewYork:SpringerPanksepp,J.
(1998).
Affectiveneuroscience:Thefoundationsofhumanandanimalemotions.
Oxford:OxfordUniversityPressParr,L.
A.
,Waller,B.
M.
,Burrows,A.
M.
,Gothard,K.
M.
&Vick,S.
J.
(2010).
MaqFACS:amuscle-basedfacialmovementcodingsystemforthemacaquemonkey.
AmericanJournalofPhysicalAnthropology,143,625–630.
doi:10.
1002/ajpa.
21401Paul,E.
S.
(2000).
Empathywithanimalsandwithhumans:aretheylinkedAnthrozos,13,194–202.
doi:10.
2752/089279300786999699Plutchik,R.
(2001).
Thenatureofemotions.
AmericanScientist,89,344–350.
doi:10.
1511/2001.
4.
344Poldrack,R.
A.
&Mumford,J.
A.
(2009).
IndependenceinROIanalysis:whereisthevoodooSocialCognitiveandAffectiveNeuroscience,4,208–213.
doi:10.
1093/scan/nsp011Pongrácz,P.
,Molnar,C.
,Miklósi,A.
&Csanyi,V.
(2005).
Humanlistenersareabletoclassifydog(Canisfamiliaris)barksrecordedindifferentsituations.
JournalofComparativePsychology,119,136–144.
doi:10.
1037/0735-7036.
119.
2.
136Preston,S.
D.
&deWaal,F.
B.
(2002).
Empathy:itsultimateandproximatebases.
BehavioralandBrainSciences,25,1–20;discussion20–71.
doi:10.
1017/S0140525X02000018Quervel-Chaumette,M.
,Dale,R.
,Marshall-Pescini,S.
&Range,F.
(2015).
Familiarityaffectsother-regardingpreferencesinpetdogs.
ScientificReports,5,18102.
doi:10.
1038/srep18102Quervel-Chaumette,M.
,Faerber,V.
,Faragó,T.
,Marshall-Pescini,S.
&Range,F.
(2016).
Investigatingempathy-likerespondingtoconspecifics'distressinpetdogs.
PlosONE,11,e0152920.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0152920Quinn,P.
C.
,Doran,M.
M.
,Reiss,J.
E.
&Hoffman,J.
E.
(2009).
Timecourseofvisualattentionininfantcategorizationofcatsversusdogs:evidenceforaheadbiasasrevealedthrougheyetracking.
ChildDevelopment,80,151–161.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1467-8624.
2008.
01251.
xRacca,A.
,Guo,K.
,Meints,K.
&Mills,D.
S.
(2012).
Readingfaces:differentiallateralgazebiasinprocessingcanineandhumanfacialexpressionsindogsand4-year-oldchildren.
PlosONE,7,e36076.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0036076AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions30Range,F.
&Viranyi,Z.
(2014).
Trackingtheevolutionaryoriginsofdog-humancooperation:the"CanineCooperationHypothesis".
FrontiersinPsychology,5,1582.
doi:10.
3389/fpsyg.
2014.
01582Range,F.
,Horn,L.
,Viranyi,Z.
&Huber,L.
(2009).
Theabsenceofrewardinducesinequityaversionindogs.
ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciencesoftheUnitedStatesofAmerica,106,340–345.
doi:10.
1073/pnas.
0810957105Range,F.
,Leitner,K.
&Virányi,Z.
(2012).
Theinfluenceoftherelationshipandmotivationoninequityaversionindogs.
SocialJusticeResearch,25,170–194.
doi:10.
1007/s11211-012-0155-xRange,F.
,Ritter,C.
&Viranyi,Z.
(2015).
Testingthemyth:tolerantdogsandaggressivewolves.
ProceedingsoftheRoyalSocietyB:BiologicalSciences,282,20150220.
doi:10.
1098/rspb.
2015.
0220Reisner,I.
R.
(1997).
Assessment,management,andprognosisofcaninedominance-relatedaggression.
VeterinaryClinicsofNorthAmerica-SmallAnimalPractice,27,479–495.
doi:10.
1016/S0195-5616(97)50050-7Reisner,I.
R.
,Mann,J.
J.
,Stanley,M.
,Huang,Y.
Y.
&Houpt,K.
A.
(1996).
Comparisonofcerebrospinalfluidmonoaminemetabolitelevelsindominant-aggressiveandnon-aggressivedogs.
BrainResearch,714,57–64.
doi:10.
1016/0006-8993(95)01464-0Rice,G.
E.
&Gainer,P.
(1962).
"Altruism"inthealbinorat.
JournalofComparativeandPhysiologicalPsychology,55,123–125.
doi:10.
3758/BF03336420Riemer,S.
,Assis,L.
,Pike,T.
W.
&Mills,D.
S.
(2016).
Dynamicchangesineartemperatureinrelationtoseparationdistressindogs.
Physiology&Behavior,167,86–91.
doi:10.
1016/j.
physbeh.
2016.
09.
002Roberts,T.
,McGreevy,P.
&Valenzuela,M.
(2010).
Humaninducedrotationandreorganizationofthebrainofdomesticdogs.
PlosONE,5,e11946.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0011946Roberts,W.
&Strayer,J.
(1996).
Empathy,emotionalexpressiveness,andprosocialbehavior.
ChildDevelopment,67,449–470.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1467-8624.
1996.
tb01745.
xRomero,T.
,Ito,M.
,Saito,A.
&Hasegawa,T.
(2014).
Socialmodulationofcontagiousyawninginwolves.
PlosONE,9,e105963.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0105963Romero,T.
,Konno,A.
&Hasegawa,T.
(2013).
Familiaritybiasandphysiologicalresponsesincontagiousyawningbydogssupportlinktoempathy.
PlosONE,8,e71365.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0071365Rooney,N.
J.
,Bradshaw,J.
W.
S.
&Robinson,I.
H.
(2000).
Acomparisonofdog-doganddog-humanplaybehaviour.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,66,235–248.
doi:10.
1016/S0168-1591(99)00078-7Rygula,R.
,Szczech,E.
,Kregiel,J.
,Golebiowska,J.
,Kubik,J.
&Popik,P.
(2015).
Cognitivejudgmentbiasinthepsychostimulant-inducedmodelofmaniainrats.
Psychopharmacology,232,651–660.
doi:10.
1007/s00213-014-3707-ySaarela,M.
V.
,Hlushchuk,Y.
,Williams,A.
C.
,Schurmann,M.
,Kalso,E.
&Hari,R.
(2007).
Thecompassionatebrain:humansdetectintensityofpainfromanother'sface.
CerebralCortex,17,230–237.
doi:10.
1093/cercor/bhj141Schilbach,L.
,Timmermans,B.
,Reddy,V.
,Costall,A.
,Bente,G.
,Schlicht,T.
&Vogeley,K.
(2013).
Towardasecond-personneuroscience.
BehavioralandBrainSciences,36,393–414.
doi:10.
1017/S0140525X12000660AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions31Schirmer,A.
,Seow,C.
S.
&Penney,T.
B.
(2013).
Humansprocessdogandhumanfacialaffectinsimilarways.
PlosONE,8,e74591.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0074591Scholl,B.
J.
&Tremoulet,P.
D.
(2000).
Perceptualcausalityandanimacy.
TrendsinCognitiveSciences,4,299–309.
doi:10.
1016/S1364-6613(00)01506-0Schürmann,M.
,Hesse,M.
D.
,Stephan,K.
E.
,Saarela,M.
,Zilles,K.
,Hari,R.
&Fink,G.
R.
(2005).
Yearningtoyawn:theneuralbasisofcontagiousyawning.
Neuroimage,24,1260–1264.
doi:10.
1016/j.
neuroimage.
2004.
10.
022Scott,J.
P.
&Fuller,J.
L.
(1965).
Geneticsandthesocialbehaviorofthedog.
Chicago,IL:TheUniversityofChicagoPress.
Senju,A.
,Kikuchi,Y.
,Akechi,H.
,Hasegawa,T.
,Tojo,Y.
&Osanai,H.
(2009).
Briefreport:doeseyecontactinducecontagiousyawninginchildrenwithautismspectrumdisorderJournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders,39,1598–1602.
doi:10.
1007/s10803-009-0785-5Senju,A.
,Maeda,M.
,Kikuchi,Y.
,Hasegawa,T.
,Tojo,Y.
&Osanai,H.
(2007).
Absenceofcontagiousyawninginchildrenwithautismspectrumdisorder.
BiologyLetters,3,706–708.
doi:10.
1098/rsbl.
2007.
0337Sforzini,E.
,Michelazzi,M.
,Spada,E.
,Ricci,C.
,Carenzi,C.
,Milani,S.
,Luzi,F.
&Verga,M.
(2009).
Evaluationofyoungandadultdogs'reactivity.
JournalofVeterinaryBehavior-ClinicalApplicationsandResearch,4,3–10.
doi:10.
1016/j.
jveb.
2008.
09.
035Shaver,P.
R.
,Wu,S.
&Schwartz,J.
C.
(1992).
Cross-culturalsimilaritiesanddifferencesinemotionanditsrepresentation:aprototypeapproach.
InM.
S.
Clark(Ed.
),Emotion(pp.
175–212).
ThousandOaks,CA:SagePublications,Inc.
Sherman,B.
L.
&Mills,D.
S.
(2008).
Canineanxietiesandphobias:anupdateonseparationanxietyandnoiseaversions.
VeterinaryClinicsofNorthAmerica-SmallAnimalPractice,38,1081–1106.
doi:10.
1016/j.
cvsm.
2008.
04.
012Silva,K.
&deSousa,L.
(2011).
'Canisempathicus'Aproposalondogs'capacitytoempathizewithhumans.
BiologyLetters,7,489–492.
doi:10.
1098/rsbl.
2011.
0083Silva,K.
,Bessa,J.
&deSousa,L.
(2012).
Auditorycontagiousyawningindomesticdogs(Canisfamiliaris):firstevidenceforsocialmodulation.
AnimalCognition,15,721–724.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-012-0473-2Silva,K.
,Bessa,J.
&deSousa,L.
(2013).
Familiarity-connectedorstress-basedcontagiousyawningindomesticdogs(Canisfamiliaris)Someadditionaldata.
AnimalCognition,16,1007–1009.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-013-0669-0Singer,T.
,Seymour,B.
,O'Doherty,J.
,Kaube,H.
,Dolan,R.
J.
&Frith,C.
D.
(2004).
Empathyforpaininvolvestheaffectivebutnotsensorycomponentsofpain.
Science,303,1157–1162.
doi:10.
1126/science.
1093535Sjaastad,O.
V.
,Sand,O.
&Hove,K.
(2010).
Physiologyofdomesticanimals.
Oslo,Norway:ScandinavianVeterinaryPress.
Somppi,S.
,Trnqvist,H.
,Kujala,M.
V.
,Hnninen,L.
,Krause,C.
M.
&Vainio,O.
(2016).
Dogsevaluatethreateningfacialexpressionsbytheirbiologicalvalidity:evidencefromgazingpatterns.
PlosOne,11,e0143047.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0143047Soproni,K.
,Miklósi,A.
,Topal,J.
&Csanyi,V.
(2001).
Comprehensionofhumancommunicativesignsinpetdogs(Canisfamiliaris).
JournalofComparativePsychology,115,122–126.
doi:10.
1037/0735-7036.
115.
2.
122AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions32Spunt,R.
P.
,Ellsworth,E.
&Adolphs,R.
(2016).
Theneuralbasisofunderstandingtheexpressionoftheemotionsinmanandanimals.
SocialCognitiveandAffectiveNeuroscience.
doi:10.
1093/scan/nsw161Tami,G.
&Gallagher,A.
(2009).
Descriptionofthebehaviourofdomesticdog(Canisfamiliaris)byexperiencedandinexperiencedpeople.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,120,159–169.
doi:10.
1016/j.
applanim.
2009.
06.
009Tate,A.
J.
,Fischer,H.
,Leigh,A.
E.
&Kendrick,K.
M.
(2006).
Behaviouralandneurophysiologicalevidenceforfaceidentityandfaceemotionprocessinginanimals.
PhilosophicalTransactionsoftheRoyalSocietyB:BiologicalSciences,361,2155–2172.
doi:10.
1098/rstb.
2006.
1937Taylor,N.
&Signal,T.
D.
(2005).
Empathyandattitudestoanimals.
Anthrozos,18,18–27.
doi:10.
2752/089279305785594342Tomarken,A.
J.
,Davidson,R.
J.
,Wheeler,R.
E.
&Doss,R.
C.
(1992).
Individualdifferencesinanteriorbrainasymmetryandfundamentaldimensionsofemotion.
JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,62,676–687.
doi:10.
1037/0022-3514.
62.
4.
676Trnqvist,H.
,Kujala,M.
V.
,Somppi,S.
,Hnninen,L.
,Pastell,M.
,Krause,C.
M.
,Kujala,J.
&Vainio,O.
(2013).
Visualevent-relatedpotentialsofdogs:anon-invasiveelectroencephalographystudy.
AnimalCognition,16,973–982.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-013-0630-2Trnqvist,H.
,Somppi,S.
,Koskela,A.
,Krause,C.
M.
,Vainio,O.
&Kujala,M.
V.
(2015).
Comparisonofdogsandhumansinvisualscanningofsocialinteraction.
RoyalSocietyOpenScience,2,150341.
doi:10.
1098/rsos.
150341Torres-Pereira,C.
M.
C.
&Broom,D.
M.
(2014).
Doesdogbehaviourandphysiologyafterdoingwrongindicateguilt,shameorfearVitoria-Gasteiz,Spain:WageningenAcademicPublishers.
Travain,T.
,Colombo,E.
S.
,Grandi,L.
C.
,Heinzl,E.
,Pelosi,A.
,PratoPrevide,E.
&Valsecchi,P.
(2016).
HowgoodisthisfoodAstudyondogs'emotionalresponsestoapotentiallypleasanteventusinginfraredthermography.
Physiology&Behavior,159,80–87.
doi:10.
1016/j.
physbeh.
2016.
03.
019Travain,T.
,Colombo,E.
S.
,Heinzl,E.
,Bellucci,D.
,PratoPrevide,E.
&Valsecchi,P.
(2015).
Hotdogs:thermographyintheassessmentofstressindogs(Canisfamiliaris)—apilotstudy.
JournalofVeterinaryBehavior:ClinicalApplicationsandResearch,10,17–23.
doi:10.
1016/j.
jveb.
2014.
11.
003Trut,L.
N.
(2001).
Experimentalstudiesofearlycaniddomestication.
InA.
Ruvinsky&J.
Sampson(Eds.
),Thegeneticsofthedog(pp.
15–42).
NewYork:CABIPublishing.
Turcsán,B.
,Szánthó,F.
,Miklósi,A.
&Kubinyi,E.
(2015).
FetchingwhattheguardianprefersDogsrecognizedisgustandhappinessinhumanbehaviour.
AnimalCognition,18,83–94.
doi:10.
1007/s10071-014-0779-3Tversky,A.
&Kahneman,D.
(1974).
Judgmentunderuncertainty:heuristicsandbiases.
Science,185,1124–1131.
doi:10.
1126/science.
185.
4157.
1124Urquiza-Haas,E.
G.
&Kotrschal,K.
(2015).
Themindbehindanthropomorphicthinking:attributionofmentalstatestootherspecies.
AnimalBehaviour,109,167–176.
doi:10.
1016/j.
anbehav.
2015.
08.
011AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions33Usui,S.
,Senju,A.
,Kikuchi,Y.
,Akechi,H.
,Tojo,Y.
,Osanai,H.
&Hasegawa,T.
(2013).
Presenceofcontagiousyawninginchildrenwithautismspectrumdisorder.
AutismResearchandTreatment,2013,971686.
doi:10.
1155/2013/971686Vaes,J.
,Meconi,F.
,Sessa,P.
&Olechowski,M.
(2016).
Minimalhumanitycuesinduceneuralempathicreactionstowardsnon-humanentities.
Neuropsychologia,89,132–140.
doi:10.
1016/j.
neuropsychologia.
2016.
06.
004vandenBerg,L.
,Schilder,M.
B.
H.
&Knol,B.
W.
(2003).
Behaviorgeneticsofcanineaggression:behavioralphenotypingofgoldenretrieversbymeansofanaggressiontest.
BehaviorGenetics,33,469–483.
doi:10.
1023/A:1025714431089vanderBorg,J.
A.
M.
,Beerda,B.
,Ooms,M.
,deSouza,A.
S.
,vanHagen,M.
&Kemp,B.
(2010).
Evaluationofbehaviourtestingforhumandirectedaggressionindogs.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,128,78–90.
doi:10.
1016/j.
applanim.
2010.
09.
016vanderBorg,J.
A.
M.
,Netto,W.
J.
&Planta,D.
J.
U.
(1991).
Behavioral-testingofdogsinanimalshelterstopredictproblembehavior.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,32,237–251.
doi:10.
1016/S0168-1591(05)80047-4Vick,S.
J.
,Waller,B.
M.
,Parr,L.
A.
,SmithPasqualini,M.
C.
&Bard,K.
A.
(2007).
Across-speciescomparisonoffacialmorphologyandmovementinhumansandchimpanzeesusingtheFacialActionCodingSystem(FACS).
JournalofNonverbalBehavior,31,1–20.
doi:10.
1007/s10919-006-0017-zVul,E.
,Harris,C.
,Winkielman,P.
&Pashler,H.
(2009).
PuzzlinglyhighcorrelationsinfMRIstudiesofemotion,personality,andsocialcognition.
PerspectivesonPsychologicalScience,4,274–290.
doi:10.
1111/j.
1745-6924.
2009.
01125.
xWalker,J.
K.
,McGrath,N.
,Handel,I.
G.
,Waran,N.
K.
&Phillips,C.
J.
C.
(2014).
DoesowningacompanionanimalinfluencethebeliefthatanimalsexperienceemotionssuchasgriefAnimalWelfare,23,71–79.
doi:10.
7120/09627286.
23.
1.
071Walker,J.
,Dale,A.
,Waran,N.
,Clarke,N.
,Farnworth,M.
&Wemelsfelder,F.
(2010).
TheassessmentofemotionalexpressionindogsusingaFreeChoiceProfilingmethodology.
AnimalWelfare,19,75–84.
Waller,B.
M.
,Kuchenbuch,P.
,Lembeck,M.
,Burrows,A.
M.
&Liebal,K.
(2012).
GibbonFACS:amusclebasedcodingsystemforthehylobatids.
InternationalJournalofPrimatology,33,809–821.
doi:10.
1007/s10764-012-9611-6Waller,B.
M.
,Peirce,K.
,Caeiro,C.
C.
,Scheider,L.
,Burrows,A.
M.
,McCune,S.
&Kaminski,J.
(2013).
Paedomorphicfacialexpressionsgivedogsaselectiveadvantage.
PlosONE,8,e82686.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0082686Wan,M.
,Bolger,N.
&Champagne,F.
A.
(2012).
Humanperceptionoffearindogsvariesaccordingtoexperiencewithdogs.
PlosONE,7,e51775.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0051775Ward,C.
,Bauer,E.
B.
&Smuts,B.
B.
(2008).
Partnerpreferencesandasymmetriesinsocialplayamongdomesticdog,Canislupusfamiliaris,littermates.
AnimalBehaviour,76,1187–1199.
doi:10.
1016/j.
anbehav.
2008.
06.
004Watanabe,S.
&Ono,K.
(1986).
Anexperimentalanalysisof"empathic"response:effectsofpainreactionsofpigeonuponotherpigeon'soperantbehavior.
BehaviouralProcesses,13,269–277.
doi:10.
1016/0376-6357(86)90089-6Wathan,J.
,Burrows,A.
M.
,Waller,B.
M.
&McComb,K.
(2015).
EquiFACS:theEquineFacialActionCodingSystem.
PlosONE,10,e0131738.
doi:10.
1371/journal.
pone.
0131738AnimalSentience2017.
013:KujalaonCanineEmotions34WestburyIngham,H.
R.
,Neumann,D.
&Waters,A.
(2015).
Empathy-relatedratingstostillimagesofhumanandnonhumananimalgroupsinnegativecontextsgradedforphylogeneticsimilarity.
Anthrozos,28,113–130.
doi:10.
2752/089279315x14129350722136Whalen,P.
J.
(1998).
Fear,vigilance,andambiguity:initialneuroimagingstudiesofthehumanamygdala.
CurrentDirectionsinPsychologicalScience,7,177–188.
doi:10.
1111/1467-8721.
ep10836912Wright,J.
C.
&Nesselrote,M.
S.
(1987).
Classificationofbehaviorproblemsindogs:distributionsofage,breed,sexandreproductivestatus.
AppliedAnimalBehaviourScience,19,169–178.
doi:10.
1016/0168-1591(87)90213-9Wright,W.
F.
&Bower,G.
H.
(1992).
Moodeffectsonsubjective-probabilityassessment.
OrganizationalBehaviorandHumanDecisionProcesses,52,276–291.
doi:10.
1016/0749-5978(92)90039-AYong,M.
H.
&Ruffman,T.
(2014).
Emotionalcontagion:dogsandhumansshowasimilarphysiologicalresponsetohumaninfantcrying.
BehaviouralProcesses,108,155–165.
doi:10.
1016/j.
beproc.
2014.
10.
006Yong,M.
H.
&Ruffman,T.
(2016).
Domesticdogsandhumaninfantslookmoreathappyandangryfacesthansadfaces.
MultisensoryResearch,29,749–771.
doi:10.
1163/22134808-00002535CallforCommentary:AnimalSentiencepublishesOpenPeerCommentaryonallacceptedtargetarticles.
Targetarticlesarepeer-reviewed.
Commentariesareeditoriallyreviewed.
Therearesubmittedcommentariesaswellasinvitedcommentaries.
Commentariesappearassoonastheyhavebeenreviewed,revisedandaccepted.
Targetarticleauthorsmayrespondtotheircommentariesindividuallyorinajointresponsetomultiplecommentaries.
Instructions:http://animalstudiesrepository.
org/animsent/guidelines.
htmlUQM/ISCCognitiveScienceSummerSchoolJune26-July62018,Montreal,CanadaTheOtherMindsProblem:AnimalSentienceandCognitionOverview.
SinceDescartes,philosophersknowthereisnowaytoknowforsurewhat—orwhether—othersfeel(noteveniftheytellyou).
Science,however,isnotaboutcertaintybutaboutprobabilityandevidence.
The7.
5billionindividualmembersofthehumanspeciescantelluswhattheyarefeeling.
Butthereare9millionotherspeciesontheplanet(20quintillionindividuals),fromelephantstojellyfish,withwhichhumanssharebiologicalandcognitiveancestry,butnotoneotherspeciescanspeak:Whichofthemcanfeel—andwhatdotheyfeelTheirhumanspokespersons—thecomparativepsychologists,ethologists,evolutionists,andcognitiveneurobiologistswhoaretheworld'sleadingexpertsin"mind-reading"otherspecies—willprovideasweepingpanoramaofwhatitfeelsliketobeanelephant,ape,whale,cow,pig,dog,bat,chicken,fish,lizard,lobster,snail:Thisgrowingbodyoffactsaboutnonhumansentiencehasprofoundimplicationsnotonlyforourunderstandingofhumancognition,butforourtreatmentofothersentientspecies.
GregoryBerns:DecodingtheDog'sMindwithAwakeNeuroimagingGordonBurghardt:ProbingtheUmweltofReptilesJonSakata:AudienceEffectsonCommunicationSignalsPANEL1:Reptiles,BirdsandMammalsWORKSHOP1:KristinAndrews:The"Other"Problems:Mind,Behavior,andAgency9SarahBrosnan:HowDoPrimatesFeelAboutTheirSocialPartnersAlexanderOphir:TheCognitiveEcologyofMonogamyMichaelHendricks:IntegratingActionandPerceptioninaSmallNervousSystemPANEL2:Primates,VolesandWormsWORKSHOP2:JonathanBirch:AnimalSentienceandthePrecautionaryPrincipleMalcolmMacIver:HowSentienceChangedAfterFishInvadedLand385MillionYearsAgoSarahWoolley:NeuralMechanismsofPreferenceinFemaleSongbirdSimonReader:AnimalSocialLearning:ImplicationsforUnderstandingOthersPANEL3:SeatoLandtoAirWORKSHOP3:StevenM.
Wise:NonhumanPersonhoodTomokoOhyama:ActionSelectioninaSmallBrain(DrosophilaMaggot)MikeRyan:"CrazyLove":NonlinearityandIrrationalityinMateChoiceLouisLefebvre:AnimalInnovation:FromEcologytoNeurotransmittersPANEL4:Maggots,FrogsandBirds:FlexibilityEvolvingSPECIALEVENT:MarioCyr:PolarBearsColinChapman:WhyDoWeWanttoThinkPeopleAreDifferentVladimirPradosudov:ChickadeeSpatialCognitionJonathanBalcombe:TheSentientWorldofFishesPANEL5:Like-MindednessandUnlike-MindednessWORKSHOP5(part1):GaryComstock:ACow'sConceptofHerFutureWORKSHOP5(part2):Jean-JacquesKona-Boun:PhysicalandMentalRiskstoCattleandHorsesinRodeosJoshuaPlotnik:ThoughtfulTrunks:ApplicationofElephantCognitionforElephantConservationLoriMarino:WhoAreDolphinsPANEL6:MammalsAll,GreatandSmallLarryYoung:TheNeurobiologyofSocialBonding,EmpathyandSocialLossinMonogamousVolesWORKSHOP6:LoriMarino:TheInconvenientTruthAboutThinkingChickensAndrewAdamatzky:SlimeMould:CognitionThroughComputationFrantisekBaluska&StefanoMancuso:WhataPlantKnowsandPerceivesArthurReber:ANovelTheoryoftheOriginofMind:ConversationsWithaCaterpillarandaBacteriumPANEL7:Microbes,MoldsandPlantsWORKSHOP7:SuzanneHeld&MichaelMendl:PigCognitionandWhyItMattersJamesSimmons:WhatIsItLikeToBeABatDebbieKelly:SpatialCognitioninFood-StoringStevePhelps:SocialCognitionAcrossSpeciesPANEL8:SocialSpaceWORKSHOP8:TobeannouncedLarsChittka:TheMindoftheBeeReuvenDukas:InsectEmotions:MechanismsandEvolutionaryBiologyAdamShriver:DoHumanLesionStudiesTellUstheCortexisRequiredforPainExperiencesPANEL9:TheInvertebrateMindWORKSHOP9:DelciannaWinders:NonhumanAnimalsinSportandEntertainmentCareltenCate:AvianCapacityforCategorizationandAbstractionJenniferMather:DoSquidHaveaSenseofSelfSteveChang:NeurobiologyofMonkeysThinkingAboutOtherMonkeysPANEL10:OthersinMindWORKSHOP10:TheLegalStatusofSentientNonhumanSpecies

LOCVPS全场8折,香港云地/邦联VPS带宽升级不加价

LOCVPS发布了7月份促销信息,全场VPS主机8折优惠码,续费同价,同时香港云地/邦联机房带宽免费升级不加价,原来3M升级至6M,2GB内存套餐优惠后每月44元起。这是成立较久的一家国人VPS服务商,提供美国洛杉矶(MC/C3)、和中国香港(邦联、沙田电信、大埔)、日本(东京、大阪)、新加坡、德国和荷兰等机房VPS主机,基于XEN或者KVM虚拟架构,均选择国内访问线路不错的机房,适合建站和远程办...

特网云-新上线香港五区补货资源充足限时抢 虚拟主机6折,低至38元!

官方网站:点击访问特网云官网活动方案:===========================香港云限时购==============================支持Linux和Windows操作系统,配置都是可以自选的,非常的灵活,宽带充足新老客户活动期间新购活动款产品都可以享受续费折扣(只限在活动期间购买活动款产品才可享受续费折扣 优惠码:AADE01),购买折扣与续费折扣不叠加,都是在原价...

织梦DEDECMS即将授权收费和维权模式 站长应对的几个方法

这两天在站长群里看到不少有使用DEDECMS织梦程序的朋友比较着急,因为前两天有看到来自DEDECMS,我们熟悉的织梦程序官方发布的公告,将会在10月25日开始全面商业用途的使用DEDECMS内容管理程序的会采用授权收费模式,如果我们有在个人或者企业商业用途的,需要联系且得到授权才可以使用,否则后面会通过维权的方式。对于这个事情,我们可能有些站长经历过,比如字体、图片的版权。以及有一些国内的CMS...

javlibrary.com为你推荐
12306崩溃为什么12306进不去安徽汽车网安徽汽车票查询冯媛甑冯媛甄 康熙来了月神谭求男变女类的变身小说同一服务器网站一个服务器放多个网站怎么设置?336.com求一个游戏的网站 你懂得www.kanav001.com长虹V001手机小游戏下载的网址是什么lcoc.toptop weenie 是什么?bbs2.99nets.com天堂1单机版到底怎么做partnersonlinecashfiesta 该怎么使用啊~~
华众虚拟主机管理系统 紧急升级请记住新域名 出租服务器 备案域名出售 3322动态域名 mach vmsnap3 创宇云 evssl 申请空间 java虚拟主机 100m免费空间 gspeed 新家坡 服务器合租 卡巴斯基免费试用 绍兴电信 路由跟踪 中国电信测速网站 atom处理器 更多